PT - JOURNAL ARTICLE AU - Bakshi, Vaishali P. AU - Geyer, Mark A. TI - Phencyclidine-Induced Deficits in Prepulse Inhibition of Startle are Blocked by Prazosin, an <em>Alpha</em>-1 Noradrenergic Antagonist DP - 1997 Nov 01 TA - Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics PG - 666--674 VI - 283 IP - 2 4099 - http://jpet.aspetjournals.org/content/283/2/666.short 4100 - http://jpet.aspetjournals.org/content/283/2/666.full SO - J Pharmacol Exp Ther1997 Nov 01; 283 AB - Prepulse inhibition (PPI) is a form of plasticity of the startle response in which presentation of a weak stimulus immediately before an intense startling stimulus reduces the resultant startle response. Deficits in PPI, an operational measure of sensorimotor gating, are observed in schizophrenia patients and can be modeled in rats by the psychotogen phencyclidine (PCP). PCP-induced deficits in PPI in rats are resistant to dopamine and serotonin antagonists but can be antagonized by antipsychotics such as clozapine, olanzapine and Seroquel. These latter antipsychotics have antagonistic actions at several receptors, including alpha-1 and alpha-2 adrenergic, M1 muscarinic and γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA)-A receptors. Although the direct actions of PCP are thought to be mediated by noncompetitive antagonism of N-methyl-D-aspartate sites, PCP thereby indirectly activates multiple neurotransmitter systems, including those affected by the aforementioned antipsychotics. The present studies examined the possibility that an antagonist action at a particular receptor subtype might be responsible for the interaction between PCP and the clozapine-like antipsychotics by testing whether a selective antagonist at alpha-1, alpha-2, M1 or GABA-A receptors would prevent the PCP-induced deficit in PPI in rats. Animals were pretreated with either the alpha-1 antagonist prazosin (0, 0.5, 1.0 or 2.5 mg/kg), the alpha-2 antagonist RX821002 (0, 0.2 or 0.4 mg/kg), the M1 muscarinic antagonist pirenzepine (0, 10 or 30 mg/kg) or the GABA-A antagonist pitrazepin (0, 1.0 or 3.0 mg/kg) and then treated with either saline or PCP (1.5 mg/kg). Because prazosin was effective in blocking the effects of PCP, an additional experiment tested the possibility that prazosin (0, 1.0 or 2.5 mg/kg) would block the PPI deficits produced by the dopamine agonist apomorphine (0 or 0.5 mg/kg). After drug administration, animals were tested in startle chambers. PCP was found repeatedly to decrease PPI. Prazosin (1.0 and 2.5 mg/kg) blocked this deficit in two separate experiments but did not increase base-line PPI levels. The effects on PPI were dissociable from changes in startle reactivity. Furthermore, prazosin did not antagonize apomorphine-induced disruptions of PPI, which suggests that the antagonism of the PCP effect was not simply due to a generalized improvement of deficient PPI. The antagonists foralpha-2, for M1 and for GABA-A receptors had no effect on base-line PPI or on PCP-induced disruptions in PPI. These findings indicate that the PPI-disruptive effect of PCP may be mediated in part by alpha-1 adrenergic receptors and that antagonism ofalpha-1 receptors may play a major role in mediating the blockade of PCP-induced deficits in PPI by certain antipsychotics. The American Society for Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics