Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current Issue
    • Fast Forward
    • Latest Articles
    • Archive
  • Information
    • Instructions to Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • FAQs
    • For Subscribers
    • Terms & Conditions of Use
    • Permissions
  • Editorial Board
  • Alerts
    • Alerts
    • RSS Feeds
  • Virtual Issues
  • Feedback
  • Other Publications
    • Drug Metabolism and Disposition
    • Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
    • Molecular Pharmacology
    • Pharmacological Reviews
    • Pharmacology Research & Perspectives
    • ASPET

User menu

  • My alerts
  • Log in
  • Log out
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
  • Other Publications
    • Drug Metabolism and Disposition
    • Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
    • Molecular Pharmacology
    • Pharmacological Reviews
    • Pharmacology Research & Perspectives
    • ASPET
  • My alerts
  • Log in
  • Log out
  • My Cart
Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current Issue
    • Fast Forward
    • Latest Articles
    • Archive
  • Information
    • Instructions to Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • FAQs
    • For Subscribers
    • Terms & Conditions of Use
    • Permissions
  • Editorial Board
  • Alerts
    • Alerts
    • RSS Feeds
  • Virtual Issues
  • Feedback
  • Visit jpet on Facebook
  • Follow jpet on Twitter
  • Follow jpet on LinkedIn
Research ArticleBEHAVIORAL PHARMACOLOGY

Antidepressant-Like Effects of κ-Opioid Receptor Antagonists in the Forced Swim Test in Rats

Stephen D. Mague, Andrea M. Pliakas, Mark S. Todtenkopf, Hilarie C. Tomasiewicz, Yan Zhang, William C. Stevens Jr., Robert M. Jones, Philip S. Portoghese and William A. Carlezon Jr.
Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics April 2003, 305 (1) 323-330; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1124/jpet.102.046433
Stephen D. Mague
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Andrea M. Pliakas
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Mark S. Todtenkopf
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Hilarie C. Tomasiewicz
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Yan Zhang
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
William C. Stevens Jr.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Robert M. Jones
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Philip S. Portoghese
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
William A. Carlezon Jr.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

We showed previously that cAMP response element-binding protein (CREB) within the nucleus accumbens (NAc) of rats regulates immobility in the forced swim test (FST), an assay used to study depression. Because CREB regulates expression of dynorphin (which acts at κ-opioid receptors) in NAc neurons, these findings raised the possibility that κ-receptors mediate immobility behaviors in the FST. Here, we report that i.c.v. administration of the κ-antagonist nor-binaltorphimine dose dependently decreased immobility in the FST, suggesting that it has antidepressant-like effects. Implicating a specific effect at κ-receptors, similar antidepressant-like effects were seen after treatment with either of two novel, structurally dissimilar κ-antagonists: 5′-guanidinonaltrindole, which was effective after i.c.v. but not systemic treatment, and 5′-acetamidinoethylnaltrindole (ANTI), which was potent and effective after systemic treatment. The behavioral effects of the κ-antagonists resembled those of tricyclic antidepressants (desipramine) and selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (fluoxetine and citalopram). Conversely, systemic administration of the κ-agonist [5α,7α,8β]-N-methyl-N-[7-[1-pyrrolidinyl]-1-oxaspiro[4.5]dec8-yl]-benzenacetamide (U-69593) dose dependently increased immobility in the FST, consistent with prodepressant-like effects. The effects of the κ-ligands in the FST were not correlated with nonspecific effects on locomotor activity. Furthermore, the most potent and effective κ-antagonist (ANTI) did not affect the rewarding impact of lateral hypothalamic brain stimulation at a dose with strong antidepressant-like effects. These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that CREB-mediated induction of dynorphin in the NAc “triggers” immobility behavior in the FST. Furthermore, they raise the possibility that κ-antagonists may have efficacy as antidepressants, but lack stimulant or reward-related effects.

The neurobiology of depression is not understood. Because most antidepressants with clinical efficacy act upon monoamines [primarily norepinephrine (NE) and serotonin (5HT)], much research on depression has focused upon interactions between these neurotransmitters and their reuptake transporters and receptor proteins. However, recent research has become progressively focused upon the intracellular mechanisms of depression and antidepressant treatments (Manji et al., 2001; Duman, 2002; Nestler et al., 2002), with the goal of developing novel therapeutics that act faster, are more efficacious, and have fewer side effects. This approach has led to the study of brain circuits typically associated with reward-related processes, including the mesolimbic dopamine (DA) system (Pliakas et al., 2001; Newton et al., 2002).

The mesolimbic DA system projects from the ventral tegmental area of the midbrain to the nucleus accumbens (NAc) of the basal forebrain, and is modulated directly and indirectly by noradrenergic and serotonergic inputs (Pasquier et al., 1977). This circuitry contributes importantly to the hedonic (rewarding) effects of food, sexual behavior, and addictive drugs (Carlezon and Wise, 1996b; Kreek and Koob, 1998; Wise, 1998). It has been proposed that disruption of DA function within the NAc causes anhedonia (reduced ability to experience reward) (Wise, 1982), a hallmark symptom of depression. Consistent with this notion, withdrawal from chronic amphetamine in rats causes decreases in extracellular concentrations of DA within the NAc that are accompanied by behavioral depression (Paulson et al., 1991). Similarly, cocaine withdrawal decreases glucose metabolism (Hammer et al., 1993) and reduces sodium currents within this region (Zhang et al., 1998). Chronic stress, a putative precipitator of depression, dramatically alters DA transmission within the NAc (Di Chiara et al., 1999). Together, these studies suggest that the NAc has an important role in the neurobiology of depression.

Recently, experience-dependent molecular adaptations that affect DA function within the NAc have been linked to the expression of depression-like signs in rats. Stress elevates the activity of the transcription factor cAMP response element-binding protein (CREB) within the NAc shell (Pliakas et al., 2001). Elevated expression of CREB within the NAc shell increases immobility in the forced swim test (FST) (Pliakas et al., 2001), a rodent model often used to study depression (Porsolt et al., 1977). The effects of CREB within the NAc seem related to its ability to regulate transcription of dynorphin, an endogenous κ-opioid receptor ligand (Carlezon et al., 1998). Indeed, the κ-antagonist nor-binaltorphimine (norBNI) attenuates the behavioral effects of elevated CREB expression within the NAc (Carlezon et al., 1998; Pliakas et al., 2001), most likely by blocking κ-opioid receptors that normally inhibit neurotransmitter release from mesolimbic DA neurons (Shippenberg and Rea, 1997; Di Chiara and Imperato, 1988; Svingos et al., 1999). Recent evidence suggests that the actions of norBNI within the NAc itself are sufficient to cause an antidepressant-like effect in the learned helplessness paradigm (Newton et al., 2002). Taken together, these data raise the possibility that CREB-mediated transcription of dynorphin within the NAc decreases DA function, which triggers signs of depression.

Because of the possible connection between dynorphin and symptoms of depression, the present study was designed to use pharmacological tools to determine whether κ-opioid receptors regulate depression-like signs in the FST. One advantage of the FST is that it identifies in rats treatments with antidepressant effects in humans (Porsolt et al., 1977; Detke et al., 1995). We reported previously (Pliakas et al., 2001) that high doses of norBNI, a well characterized κ-antagonist with long-lasting effects (Jones and Holtzman, 1992;Spanagel and Shippenberg, 1993), can increase the latency to become immobile in the FST, a putative indicator of antidepressant-like effects. A goal of the present studies was to extend this work by examining the effects of norBNI over a more complete range of doses, while using a more stringent and widely accepted method of scoring the FST (behavioral sampling; Detke et al., 1995). Furthermore, to examine whether the antidepressant-like effects of norBNI are associated with specific actions at κ-receptors, we conducted similar studies using two novel and structurally dissimilar κ-antagonists: 5′-guanidinonaltrindole (GNTI) (Jones and Portoghese, 2000; Jewett et al., 2001; Negus et al., 2002) and 5′-acetamidinoethylnaltrindole (ANTI) (Stevens et al., 2000). We also explored the possibility that the κ-agonist [5α,7α,8β]-N-methyl-N-[7-[1-pyrrolidinyl]-1-oxaspiro[4.5]dec8-yl]-benzenacetamide (U-69593) would have the opposite (prodepressant-like) effects on behavior in the FST. In parallel, we examined the effects of each agent on locomotor activity. Finally, because systemic ANTI had strong antidepressant-like effects in the FST, we examined whether it has reward-related effects using intracranial self-stimulation (ICSS) (Wise, 1998).

Materials and Methods

Rats.

A total of 591 male Sprague-Dawley rats (Charles River Laboratories, Inc., Wilmington, MA) were used in this study. Rats used for forced swim testing or locomotor activity testing were housed in groups of four and weighed 325 to 375 g at the time of testing, whereas those used for ICSS testing were housed singly and weighed 350 to 400 g at the time of stereotaxic surgery. All rats were maintained on a 12-h light (7:00 AM–7:00 PM)/12-h dark cycle with free access to food and water except during testing. Experiments were conducted in accordance with the 1996 Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (National Institutes of Health) and McLean Hospital policies.

Drugs.

Desipramine HCl (DMI), fluoxetine HCl (FLX), cocaine HCl (COC), norBNI, and U-69593 were obtained from Sigma-Aldrich (St. Louis, MO). Citalopram (CIT) was obtained from Forest Laboratories (New York, NY). GNTI and ANTI were synthesized at the University of Minnesota. Dosages of all drugs were based on their salt form. DMI, FLX, norBNI, and GNTI were dissolved in distilled water, and COC and CIT were dissolved in saline. U-69593 was dissolved in 0.1 N acetic acid diluted with distilled water. All drugs were administered in a volume of 1 ml/kg, except for FLX (and the FLX-associated vehicle groups), which was administered in a volume of 2 ml/kg because of poor solubility.

FST.

Three hundred seventy one rats were used for studies of standard psychotropic agents (DMI, FLX, CIT, and COC) and κ-ligands (norBNI, GNTI, ANTI, and U-69593) in the FST. The FST is a 2-day procedure in which rats swim under conditions in which escape is not possible. On the 1st day, the rats are forced to swim for 15 min. The rats initially struggle to escape from the water, but eventually they adopt a posture of immobility in which they make only the movements necessary to keep their heads above water. When the rats are retested 24 h later, immobility is increased. Treatment with standard antidepressant drugs within the 24-h period between the first exposure to forced swimming and retesting can block facilitated immobility, an effect correlated with antidepressant efficacy in humans (Porsolt et al., 1977; Detke et al., 1995).

No treatment was given before the 1st day of the FST for studies involving the standard psychotropic agents, ANTI, or U-69593. For the studies involving norBNI and GNTI, each rat was anesthetized with i.p. pentobarbital (65 mg/kg) and given s.c. atropine sulfate (0.25 mg/kg) to reduce bronchial secretions. Microinjections (i.c.v., 0.3 mm posterior to bregma, 1.2 mm lateral from midsaggital suture, and 4.0 mm below dura; Paxinos and Watson, 1986) of norBNI (1.25–20 μg) or GNTI (5.0–20 μg) were administered in 2.0 μl over 10 min using a Hamilton syringe with a 26-gauge needle (Pliakas et al., 2001). At the dosages used, a single treatment with these agents has extended efficacy: norBNI produces a κ-receptor-specific blockade in rats (Jones and Holtzman, 1992) for >3 weeks (Spanagel and Shippenberg, 1993). GNTI has detectable κ-receptor-specific effects in monkeys for ∼10 days (Negus et al., 2002), although its effects in rats do not seem to endure as long as than those of norBNI (Jewett et al., 2001). (ANTI was not tested in i.c.v. studies because it is a novel compound with modifications to its chemical structure that made it hypothetically more bioavailable by systemic injection than norBNI or GNTI). Rats recovered from surgery for 2 days before the FST.

On the 1st day of the FST, rats were placed in clear, 65-cm-tall by 25-cm-diameter cylinders filled to 48 cm with 25°C water. After 15 min of forced swimming, the rats were removed from the water, dried with towels, and placed in a warmed enclosure for 30 min. The cylinders were emptied and cleaned between rats. Rats tested with the standard psychotropic agents, ANTI, or U-69593 received three separate i.p. injections at 1, 19, and 23 h after the first exposure to forced swimming, a commonly used treatment regimen (Porsolt et al., 1977;Detke et al., 1995; Carlezon et al., 2002). Rats given norBNI, GNTI, or vehicle by i.c.v. microinjection did not receive systemic injections. There were 7 to 15 rats per treatment; control (vehicle-treated) groups had the highest numbers because each daily test session included four rats, and each group of four rats contained at least one control rat.

At 24 h after the forced swim, rats were retested for 5 min (300 s) under identical swim conditions. Retest sessions were videotaped from the side of the cylinders and scored using a behavioral sampling method (Detke et al., 1995; Carlezon et al., 2002) by raters unaware of the treatment condition. Rats were rated at 5-s intervals throughout the duration of the retest session; at each 5-s interval, the predominant behavior was assigned to one of four categories: immobility, swimming, climbing, or diving. A rat was judged to be immobile if it was making only movements necessary to keep its head above water; climbing if it was making forceful thrashing movements with its forelimbs directed against the walls of the cylinder; swimming if it was actively making swimming movements that caused it to move within the center of the cylinder; and diving if it swam below the water, toward the bottom of the cylinder (data quantifying diving behavior are not shown in the present report because it rarely occurred, and it was not affected by any of the treatments tested). This behavioral sampling method reportedly differentiates classes of antidepressant drugs: for example, tricyclic antidepressants decrease immobility and increase climbing without affecting swimming, whereas selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) decrease immobility and increase swimming without affecting climbing (Detke et al., 1995).

Data from each agent were analyzed separately. The number of occurrences (to a maximum of 60) of each category of behavior was analyzed using separate one-way (treatment) analyses of variance (ANOVAs). Significant effects were analyzed using post hoc Fisher's least significant difference (LSD) tests.

Locomotor Activity.

Two hundred twelve rats were used to determine whether the treatments examined in the FST studies alter locomotor activity. These studies were conducted exactly as the FST studies had been conducted until the point of retesting, that is, all rats underwent the 1st day of the FST and were treated with the standard agents ANTI or U-69593 at the normal pretreatment times (1, 19, and 23 h after swimming). Rats given norBNI or GNTI received i.c.v. microinjections 2 days before the 1st day of the FST. At 24 h after the first exposure to forced swimming, the rats were placed for 1 h in automated 68 × 21 × 21 cm (length × width × height) activity chambers (MED Associates, St. Albans, VT) instead of being retested in the FST. There were 7 to 12 rats per treatment; control (vehicle-treated) groups had the highest numbers because each daily test session included four rats, and each group of four rats contained at least one control rat.

The total number of activity counts (photocell beam breaks) during the test session was quantified, and data for each agent were analyzed separately. For locomotor activity studies in which multiple doses of drug were tested (i.e., the standard psychotropic agents and U-69593), differences among treatment groups were analyzed using one-way (treatment) ANOVAs. Significant effects were analyzed further using post hoc Fisher's LSD tests. For studies involving norBNI, GNTI, and ANTI, only the highest active dose was tested, and differences from vehicle-treated rats were analyzed using Student's t tests.

ICSS.

Each of eight rats was anesthetized as described above and implanted with a monopolar, stainless steel electrode (0.250 mm in diameter; Plastics One, Roanoke, VA) aimed at the left medial forebrain bundle, at the level of the lateral hypothalamus (2.8 mm posterior to bregma, 1.7 mm lateral from midsaggital suture, and 7.8 mm below dura;Paxinos and Watson, 1986). The electrodes were coated with polyamide insulation except at the flattened tip. Skull screws (one of which served as the ground) and the electrode were secured to the skull with dental acrylic.

After at least a 1-week recovery, the rats were trained on a continuous reinforcement schedule (fixed ratio 1) to respond for brain stimulation, using procedures described previously (Carlezon and Wise, 1996a). Each lever press earned a 0.5-s train of square-wave cathodal pulses (0.1-ms pulse duration) at a set frequency of 158 Hz. The delivery of the stimulation was accompanied by the illumination of a 2-W house light. Responses during the 0.5-s stimulation period did not earn additional stimulation. The stimulation current (100–300 μA) was adjusted gradually to the lowest value that would sustain a reliable rate of responding (at least 40 rewards/min). Once an appropriate current was found for each rat, it was held constant throughout the remainder of the experiment.

Each rat was then adapted to brief tests with each of a descending series of 15 stimulation frequencies. Each series comprised 1-min test trials at each frequency. For each frequency tested, there was an initial 5-s “priming” phase during which noncontingent stimulation was given, followed by a 50-s test phase during which the number of responses was counted. After the test phase, there was a 5-s time out period during which no stimulation was available. The stimulation frequency was then lowered by approximately 10% (0.05 log10 units), and another trial was started. After responding had been evaluated at each of the 15 frequencies, the procedure was repeated such that each rat was given six such series per day (90 min of training). During the training procedure, the range of frequencies was adjusted for each rat so that only the highest five to six frequencies would sustain responding. To characterize the functions relating response strength to reward magnitude, a least-squares line of best fit was plotted across the frequencies that sustained responding at 20, 30, 40, 50, and 60% of the maximum rate using customized analysis software (Steven Cabilio, Montreal, QC, Canada;scabilio{at}hotmail.com). ICSS threshold was defined as the frequency at which the line intersected the x-axis (θ-0;Miliaressis et al., 1986). Drug testing started when mean ICSS thresholds varied by less than 10% over three consecutive training sessions.

For drug testing, three rate-frequency functions (“curves”) were determined for each rat immediately before drug treatment. The first curve served as a warm-up period and was discarded because it tended to be unreliable. The second and third curves were averaged to obtain the baseline (threshold and maximal response rates) parameters. Each rat then received an i.p. injection of drug or vehicle, and four more 15-min rate-frequency curves were obtained (1 h of testing). All rats received the same treatments in a standardized order: the first test was with vehicle, the second test was with cocaine (2.5 mg/kg i.p.), the third test was with vehicle, and the fourth test was with ANTI (3.0 mg/kg i.p.). Vehicle treatments were used to ensure that the rats had recovered from prior treatment with cocaine and to minimize the possibility of conditioned drug effects. It has been established previously that repeated treatment with cocaine (or other stimulants) does not cause progressive changes in drug sensitivity in the ICSS assay (for review, see Carlezon et al., 2001).

Effects of vehicle, COC, and ANTI on thresholds and maximal response rates over the 60-min test period were evaluated using one-way ANOVAs with repeated measures. The first and second tests with vehicle did not differ and were combined into a single group. Significant effects were analyzed further using post hoc Fisher's LSD tests.

Histology.

Rats that received i.c.v. microinjections or ICSS electrodes were overdosed with pentobarbital (130 mg/kg i.p.) and perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde. The fixed brains were sliced in 40-μm sections for cresyl violet staining to confirm i.c.v. microinjection and electrode placements.

Results

Each of the standard psychotropic agents (DMI, FLX, CIT, and COC) reduced immobility in the FST, but effects on the occurrences of swimming and climbing behaviors differed among drugs (Table1). DMI (a tricyclic antidepressant) dose dependently decreased occurrences of immobility (F4,61 = 6.97, P < 0.01), did not affect occurrences of swimming, and increased occurrences of climbing (F4,61 = 6.78,P < 0.01). In locomotor activity studies, DMI decreased activity counts at the highest doses tested (F4,42 = 9.21, P < 0.01). In contrast, FLX (an SSRI) dose dependently decreased occurrences of immobility (F3,45 = 4.58, P < 0.01) and increased occurrences of swimming behavior (F3,45 = 5.64,P < 0.01), but did not affect occurrences of climbing. FLX also decreased locomotor activity at doses with efficacy in the FST (F4,42 = 9.21, P < 0.01). CIT (an SSRI) had effects similar to those seen with FLX: it dose dependently decreased occurrences of immobility (F3,33 = 2.94, P < 0.05) and increased occurrences of swimming behavior (F3,33 = 4.55, P < 0.01), without affecting occurrences of climbing. CIT began to decrease locomotor activity at doses below those with efficacy in the FST (F3,30 = 4.66, P < 0.01). Like each of the SSRIs, COC dose dependently decreased occurrences of immobility (F3,36 = 7.06, P < 0.01) and increased occurrences of swimming behavior (F3,36 = 5.86,P < 0.01), without affecting occurrences of climbing. Unlike any of the other standard antidepressant agents tested, COC did not cause even nominal changes in locomotor activity at doses with efficacy in the FST. Higher doses of COC were not tested in the FST because they caused substantial increases in locomotor activity (5.0 mg/kg, 3479.3 ± 266.7; 10 mg/kg, 4971.8 ± 532.2) in small numbers of rats (n = 4, each group).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1

Effect of psychotropic drugs in the FST and on horizontal locomotion in activity chambers

The κ-antagonists (Fig. 1) produced effects in the FST that were qualitatively similar to those seen with the standard psychotropic agents. The i.c.v. microinjections of norBNI dose dependently decreased occurrences of immobility (F5,49 = 4.68, P < 0.01) and increased occurrences of swimming behavior (F5,49 = 3.85, P < 0.01) (Fig. 2a). There was also a statistically significant effect on climbing (F5,49 = 2.74, P < 0.05), but post hoc analyses revealed that this was due to differences between rats in the 1.25-μg norBNI and 10- and 20-μg norBNI groups rather than any differences from vehicle-treated rats. There was no effect of norBNI on locomotor activity at the dose with antidepressant-like effects (20 μg) (Fig. 2b). Similarly, i.c.v. microinjections of GNTI dose dependently decreased occurrences of immobility (F3,35 = 3.00,P < 0.05) and increased occurrences of swimming behavior (F3,35 = 3.04,P < 0.05), but did not affect climbing behavior (Fig.3a). GNTI had no effect on locomotor activity at the highest dose (20 μg) with antidepressant-like effects in the FST (Fig. 3b). Systemic administration of GNTI (1.0–10 mg/kg i.p.) had no effect in small numbers of rats (∼5/group) (data not shown). However, systemic (i.p.) administration of ANTI had efficacy in the FST: it dose dependently decreased occurrences of immobility (F4,44 = 2.89, P < 0.05), but unlike the other κ-antagonists, it increased occurrences of climbing behavior (F4,44 = 2.88,P < 0.05) without affecting swimming behavior (Fig.4a). Systemic administration of ANTI did not affect locomotor activity at the highest dose (3.0 mg/kg) with antidepressant-like effects in the FST (Fig. 4b).

Figure 1
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1

Chemical structures of the κ-antagonists (norBNI, GNTI, and ANTI) and the κ-agonist (U-69593) used in the present study.

Figure 2
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2

Effects of norBNI (i.c.v.) on behavior. a, treatment with norBNI in the FST decreased occurrences of immobility and increased occurrences of swimming, without affecting climbing (means ± S.E.M.). Testing began 3 days after i.c.v. injections, which is well within the known duration of norBNI effects. ∗,P < 0.05; ∗∗, P < 0.01 compared with vehicle, Fisher's LSD tests, 7 to 15 rats per group. b, norBNI did not affect locomotor activity (means ± S.E.M.) at a dose with antidepressant-like effects in the FST (Student'st tests, 7–12 rats/group).

Figure 3
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3

Effects of GNTI (i.c.v.) on behavior. a, treatment with GNTI in the FST decreased occurrences of immobility and increased occurrences of swimming, without affecting climbing (means ± S.E.M.). Testing began 3 days after i.c.v. injections. ∗,P < 0.05; ∗∗, P < 0.01 compared with vehicle, Fisher's LSD tests, 7 to 15 rats per group. b, GNTI did not affect locomotor activity (means ± S.E.M.) at a dose with antidepressant-like effects in the FST (Student'st tests, 7–12 rats/group).

Figure 4
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4

Effects of ANTI (i.p.) on behavior. a, treatment with ANTI in the FST decreased occurrences of immobility and increased occurrences of climbing, without affecting swimming (means ± S.E.M.). ∗, P < 0.05; ∗∗,P < 0.01 compared with vehicle, Fisher's LSD tests, 7 to 10 rats per group. b, ANTI did not affect locomotor activity (means ± S.E.M.) at a dose with antidepressant-like effects in the FST (Student's t tests, 7–12 rats/group).

Systemic (i.p.) administration of the κ-agonist U-69593 (Fig. 1) had effects in the FST that were qualitatively opposite to those seen with the κ-antagonists. U-69593 dose dependently increased occurrences of immobility (F5,60 = 3.97,P < 0.01) and decreased occurrences of swimming behavior (F5,60 = 4.31,P < 0.01), without affecting climbing behavior (Fig.5a). U-69593 significantly decreased locomotor activity (F2,20 = 16.34,P < 0.01) at the highest dose (10 mg/kg i.p.) with prodepressant-like effects in the FST, but had no effect at a lower dose (3.0 mg/kg) that also affected immobility and swimming behaviors (Fig. 5b).

Figure 5
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5

Effects of U-69593 (i.p.) on behavior. a, treatment with U-69593 in the FST increased occurrences of immobility and decreased occurrences of swimming, without affecting climbing (means ± S.E.M.). b, highest dose of U-69593 tested (10 mg/kg) decreased locomotor activity (means ± S.E.M.), but a lower dose that also had prodepressant-like effects in the FST (3.0 mg/kg) did not affect activity. ∗, P < 0.05; ∗∗,P < 0.01 compared with vehicle, Fisher's LSD tests, 7 to 10 rats per group.

In the ICSS studies, the effects on thresholds depended upon treatment (F2,14 = 11.8, P < 0.01), whereas there were no significant effects on maximal response rates. Treatment with vehicle tended to cause small, nonsignificant increases in ICSS thresholds (Fig. 6a) and decreases in maximum response rates (Fig. 6b) during the 1-h test session. There were no differences between the effects of the first (test 1) and second (test 3) injection with vehicle, so these two sessions were combined into a single mean. The lowest dose of cocaine (2.5 mg/kg) tested with antidepressant-like effects in the FST significantly decreased ICSS thresholds without significantly affecting maximal response rates. In contrast, the highest dose of ANTI (3.0 mg/kg) tested with antidepressant-like effects in the FST did not affect ICSS thresholds or maximal response rates. ICSS electrodes were located in the medial forebrain bundle at the level of the lateral hypothalamus, and the placements were indistinguishable from those depicted previously (Carlezon and Wise, 1996a; Carlezon et al., 2001).

Figure 6
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6

Effect of various treatments on ICSS behavior. Rats were given four treatments in a standardized order (vehicle during the first test, cocaine during the second test, vehicle during the third test, and ANTI during the fourth test). a, treatment with vehicle tended to cause small, nonsignificant increases in ICSS thresholds (mean ± S.E.M.) over the course of the 1-h test sessions. There was no difference between the two vehicle treatments, so the data were combined into a single mean. Cocaine significantly decreased ICSS thresholds at the lowest dose tested with antidepressant-like effects in the FST, whereas ANTI had no effect at the highest dose tested with antidepressant-like effects in the FST. b, none of the treatments affected response capabilities (maximal rates). ∗∗,P < 0.01 compared with vehicle, Fisher's LSD tests, eight rats per group.

Discussion

Standard antidepressants (DMI, FLX, and CIT) reduced immobility in the FST, a putative antidepressant-like effect (Porsolt et al., 1977;Detke et al., 1995; Carlezon et al., 2002). Moreover, the behavioral sampling scoring method (Detke et al., 1995) distinguishes among classes of drugs: as expected, DMI decreased immobility and increased climbing, whereas FLX and CIT each decreased immobility and increased swimming. Together, these findings confirm the sensitivity of our methodology to agents with antidepressant efficacy in humans.

Cocaine also reduced immobility in the FST, consistent with the antidepressant and “mood-elevating” effects of the drug (Post et al., 1974). Furthermore, cocaine increased swimming behaviors without affecting climbing behaviors. This pattern suggests that the antidepressant-like effects of cocaine resemble those of SSRIs more closely than those of tricyclic antidepressants, although the possibility that it reflects a dopaminergic mechanism cannot be discounted. Cocaine is often regarded as a prototypical dopaminergic agent, but it actually has higher affinity for 5HT transporters than for DA or NE transporters (Ritz and Kuhar, 1989). Additional work with agents with improved selectivity is needed to determine whether relatively specific increases in individual neurotransmitters are associated with particular behavioral patterns in the FST.

A single i.c.v. treatment with the κ-antagonist norBNI dose dependently decreased immobility in the FST, suggesting that this agent has antidepressant-like effects. This effect is consistent with our previous observations that norBNI decreases dysphoria likely associated with acute cocaine withdrawal (Carlezon et al., 1998; Pliakas et al., 2001). Indeed, antidepressants such as DMI reportedly reduce symptoms of cocaine withdrawal in humans (Gawin et al., 1989) and rats (Markou et al., 1992). Likewise, a single i.c.v. treatment with GNTI, a structurally dissimilar κ-antagonist, had antidepressant-like effects. GNTI was more potent than norBNI, consistent with previous reports in which the effects of these agents were compared (Jones and Portoghese, 2000; Jewett et al., 2001; Negus et al., 2002). The fact that structurally dissimilar κ-antagonists have similar actions in the FST suggests that the antidepressant-like effects of norBNI are attributable to a specific blockade of κ-receptors rather than nonspecific side effects. However, GNTI was not effective by systemic administration at doses up to 10 mg/kg i.p., suggesting that the chemical structure of this agent, specifically, the high pKa of the guanidinium group, may limit its bioavailability.

We also tested ANTI, a more lipophilic κ-antagonist, using the repeated systemic (i.p.) administration regimen. ANTI is a potent and selective κ-selective antagonist that contains an amidinium group, which is less basic than the guanidinium group of GNTI. This modification allows a greater percentage of nonionized ANTI to enter the brain. ANTI also contains three additional hydrophobic groups (two methylenes and one methyl) that increase its lipophilic character, further contributing to improved access to the brain and greater potency. Like norBNI and GNTI, ANTI reduced immobility in the FST, providing additional evidence that antidepressant-like effects may be a general attribute of κ-antagonists.

Effects on swimming and climbing behaviors differed among the κ-ligands. Treatment with norBNI and GNTI increased swimming behaviors, whereas ANTI increased climbing behaviors. The significance of this observation is unclear. These data suggest that alterations in the chemical structure of κ-antagonists can alter their effects on monoamine function (Detke et al., 1995). However, differential effects on the swimming and climbing measures may also involve factors other than interactions among NE, 5HT, and DA (Carlezon et al., 2002). Furthermore, systemic treatment with a κ-agonist had effects in the FST that were opposite to those seen with the antagonists: U-69593 increased immobility and decreased swimming. These data suggest that U-69593 has prodepressant-like effects in rats, which is consistent with previous observations that κ-agonists produce dysphoria in humans (Pfeiffer et al., 1986). Considering together the effects of the κ-agonist and the antagonists, these findings raise the possibility that κ-opioid receptors mediate signs of immobility in the FST.

One concern when using the FST is that nonspecific treatment effects on activity levels could complicate data interpretation. If treatments increase activity, they could seem to reduce immobility in the FST and thus be incorrectly identified as antidepressants. Accordingly, we conducted locomotion studies in parallel with the FST studies to identify potentially confounding effects. Locomotion studies were conducted exactly as the FST studies, except during the 2nd day of testing the rats were placed in activity chambers rather than being reexposed to forced swimming. If anything, the standard antidepressants tended to decrease locomotor activity at the time of retesting in the FST. It is unlikely, however, that the ability of an agent to decrease locomotor activity is necessary to cause antidepressant-like actions in the FST, because cocaine had antidepressant-like actions without affecting activity. Furthermore, other treatments have been shown to produce antidepressant-like (or prodepressant-like) effects in the FST without affecting locomotor activity (Pliakas et al., 2001). The observation that cocaine had antidepressant-like effects in the FST without affecting locomotor activity suggests that its mood-enhancing effects occur at lower doses than its stimulant effects. Similarly, none of the κ-antagonists affected locomotor activity at the highest doses with antidepressant-like effects in the FST. Although U-69593 decreased activity at the highest dose tested (10 mg/kg i.p.), it did not affect activity at a lower dose (3.0 mg/kg i.p.) that also had prodepressant-like effects. Because both antidepressant-like effects and prodepressant-like effects can be observed after treatments that decrease locomotion, it seems unlikely that our FST studies were affected by nonspecific treatment effects on locomotor activity.

Although the mechanisms by which κ-receptor agents regulate behavior in the FST are unknown, previous work suggests that altered DA function may be involved. One possibility is that immobility is mediated by κ-receptors located on the terminals of mesolimbic DA neurons that project to the NAc (Svingos et al., 1999). These presynaptic κ-receptors regulate DA release: administration of κ-agonists directly into the NAc decreases local extracellular concentrations of DA to ∼50% of baseline (Di Chiara and Imperato, 1988;Maisonneuve et al., 1994). Conversely, direct administration of κ-antagonists into the NAc increases DA concentrations to ∼175% of baseline (Maisonneuve et al., 1994). These increases in concentrations of DA are modest compared with those observed after treatment with psychostimulants such as cocaine (to 400–800% baseline) or amphetamine (to >1000% baseline) (Di Chiara and Imperato, 1988;Maisonneuve et al., 1994). Modest increases in DA concentrations within the NAc may be sufficient to cause antidepressant-like effects in the FST without stimulating locomotor activity.

Another possibility is that the efficacy of κ-antagonists in the FST is related to their ability to block the effects of experience-dependent elevations in endogenous dynorphin levels. Forced swimming activates CREB within the NAc (Pliakas et al., 2001), and CREB regulates dynorphin expression within the NAc and related tissues (Cole et al., 1995; Carlezon et al., 1998). Thus, forced swimming may trigger CREB-mediated increases in dynorphin transcription within the NAc, which contribute to the facilitated immobility normally observed during the second exposure to the FST (Porsolt et al., 1977; Pliakas et al., 2001). As such, κ-antagonists may have antidepressant effects by blocking dynorphin-mediated reductions in extracellular concentrations of DA within the NAc, an effect associated with symptoms of depression, including anhedonia (Wise, 1982; Paulson et al., 1991, Pliakas et al., 2001). Regardless of the exact mechanisms, recent evidence suggests that κ-antagonist actions within the NAc itself are sufficient to cause an antidepressant-like effect (Newton et al., 2002).

If κ-antagonists affect behavior through their ability to promote DA transmission in the NAc then an obvious concern is that they may have abuse liability (Di Chiara and Imperato, 1988). Drugs of abuse facilitate ICSS in rats, reducing the amounts of stimulation required to sustain responding (thresholds) (Wise, 1998). In the present study, cocaine significantly reduced ICSS thresholds at a dose (2.5 mg/kg i.p.) with antidepressant-like effects in the FST, confirming that its “mood-elevating” effects are associated with reward-related effects that contribute to abuse liability. In contrast, ANTI did not affect ICSS thresholds at a dose with antidepressant-like effects in the FST. Thus, the effects of ANTI seem similar to those of other antidepressant drugs (e.g., DMI and FLX), which, if anything, initially increase ICSS thresholds (Hall et al., 1990; Markou et al., 1992; Lee and Kornetsky, 1998). The fact that effective doses of ANTI lack stimulant effects in locomotor activity studies and rewarding effects in ICSS studies is early evidence that this agent lacks abuse liability at therapeutic concentrations. Studies examining the effects of ANTI and other κ-ligands on ICSS behavior across a wide range of doses are in progress.

The present study raises the possibility that κ-antagonists may be a new approach to the treatment of depressive disorders. A limitation of norBNI and GNTI is low bioavailability after systemic administration. Structural modifications designed to reduce basicity and increase lipophilicity led to the design of ANTI (Stevens et al., 2000), which was efficacious in the FST after systemic administration. Additional modifications may yield κ-antagonists with increased potency or efficacy and provide additional insight into the therapeutic potential of this class of agents.

Acknowledgments

We thank Forest Laboratories for the gift of citalopram, Michael Detke for the use of behavioral sampling software, and Sonia Guediche for technical assistance.

Footnotes

  • This study was funded by Grant MH63266 from the National Institute of Mental Health (to W.C.), Grant DA01533 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (to P.S.P.), an unrestricted gift from Johnson and Johnson (to W.C.), and donations by John A. Kaneb (to W.C.).

  • DOI: 10.1124/jpet.102.046433

  • Abbreviations:
    NE
    norepinephrine
    5HT
    5-hydroxytryptamine
    DA
    dopamine
    NAc
    nucleus accumbens
    CREB
    cAMP response element-binding protein
    FST
    forced swim test
    norBNI
    nor-binaltorphimine
    GNTI
    5′-guanidinonaltrindole
    ANTI
    5′-acetamidinoethylnaltrindole
    U-69593
    (5α,7α,8β)-N-methyl-N-(7-[1-pyrrolidinyl]-1-oxaspiro[4.5]dec8-yl)-benzenacetamide
    ICSS
    intracranial self-stimulation
    DMI
    desipramine
    FLX
    fluoxetine
    COC
    cocaine
    CIT
    citalopram
    SSRI
    selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor
    ANOVA
    analysis of variance
    LSD
    least significant difference
    • Received November 1, 2002.
    • Accepted January 3, 2003.
  • The American Society for Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics

References

  1. ↵
    1. Carlezon WA, Jr,
    2. Pliakas AM,
    3. Parrow AM,
    4. Detke MJ,
    5. Cohen BM,
    6. Renshaw PF
    (2002) Antidepressant-like effects of cytidine in the forced swim test in rats. Biol Psychiatry 51:882–889.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Carlezon WA, Jr,
    2. Thome J,
    3. Olson V,
    4. Lane-Ladd SB,
    5. Brodkin ES,
    6. Hiroi N,
    7. Duman RS,
    8. Neve RL,
    9. Nestler EJ
    (1998) Regulation of cocaine reward by CREB. Science (Wash DC) 282:2272–2275.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    1. Carlezon WA, Jr,
    2. Todtenkopf MS,
    3. McPhie DL,
    4. Pimental P,
    5. Pliakas AM,
    6. Stellar JR,
    7. Trzcinska M
    (2001) Repeated exposure to rewarding brain stimulation down-regulates GluR1 expression in the ventral tegmental area. Neuropsychopharmacology 25:232–241.
    OpenUrl
  4. ↵
    1. Carlezon WA, Jr,
    2. Wise RA
    (1996a) Microinjections of phencyclidine (PCP) and related drugs into nucleus accumbens shell potentiate medial forebrain bundle brain stimulation reward. Psychopharmacology 128:413–420.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Carlezon WA, Jr,
    2. Wise RA
    (1996b) Rewarding actions of phencyclidine and related drugs in nucleus accumbens shell and frontal cortex. J Neurosci 16:3112–3122.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Cole RL,
    2. Konradi C,
    3. Douglass J,
    4. Hyman SE
    (1995) Neuronal adaptation to amphetamine and dopamine: molecular mechanisms of prodynorphin gene regulation in rat striatum. Neuron 14:813–823.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Detke MJ,
    2. Rickels M,
    3. Lucki I
    (1995) Active behaviors in the rat forced swimming test differentially produced by serotonergic and noradrenergic antidepressants. Psychopharmacology 121:66–72.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Di Chiara G,
    2. Imperato A
    (1988) Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 85:5274–5278.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Di Chiara G,
    2. Loddo P,
    3. Tanda G
    (1999) Reciprocal changes in prefrontal and limbic dopamine responsiveness to aversive and rewarding stimuli after chronic mild stress: implications for the psychobiology of depression. Biol Psychiatry 46:1624–1633.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Duman RS
    (2002) Synaptic plasticity and mood disorders. Mol Psychiatry 7:S29–S34.
    OpenUrl
  11. ↵
    1. Gawin FH,
    2. Kleber HD,
    3. Byck R,
    4. Rounsaville BJ,
    5. Kosten TR,
    6. Jatlow PI,
    7. Morgan C
    (1989) Desipramine facilitation of initial cocaine abstinence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 46:117–121.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Hall FS,
    2. Stellar JR,
    3. Kelley AE
    (1990) Acute and chronic desipramine treatment effects on rewarding electrical stimulation of the lateral hypothalamus. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 37:277–281.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Hammer RP, Jr,
    2. Pires WS,
    3. Markou A,
    4. Koob GF
    (1993) Withdrawal following cocaine self-administration decreases regional cerebral metabolic rate in critical brain reward regions. Synapse 14:73–80.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Jewett DC,
    2. Grace MK,
    3. Jones RM,
    4. Billington CJ,
    5. Portoghese PS,
    6. Levine AS
    (2001) The κ-opioid antagonist GNTI reduces U50,488-, DAMGO- and deprivation-induced feeding, but not butorphanol- and neuropeptide Y-induced feeding in rats. Brain Res 909:75–80.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Jones DN,
    2. Holtzman SG
    (1992) Long term κ-opioid receptor blockade following nor-binaltorphimine. Eur J Pharmacol 215:345–348.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Jones RM,
    2. Portoghese PS
    (2000) 5′-Guanidinonaltrindole, a highly selective and potent κ-opioid receptor antagonist. Eur J Pharmacol 396:49–52.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Kreek MJ,
    2. Koob GF
    (1998) Drug dependence: stress and dysregulation of brain reward pathways. Drug Alcohol Depend 51:23–47.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Lee K,
    2. Kornetsky C
    (1998) Acute and chronic fluoxetine treatment decreases the sensitivity of rats to rewarding brain stimulation. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 60:539–544.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Maisonneuve IM,
    2. Archer S,
    3. Glick SD
    (1994) U50,488, a κ-opioid receptor agonist, attenuates cocaine-induced increases in extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens of rats. Neurosci Lett 181:57–60.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Manji H,
    2. Drevets WC,
    3. Charney DS
    (2001) The cellular neurobiology of depression. Nat Med 7:541–547.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Markou A,
    2. Hauger RL,
    3. Koob GF
    (1992) Desmethylimipramine attenuates cocaine withdrawal in rats. Psychopharmacology 109:305–314.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Miliaressis E,
    2. Rompre PP,
    3. Laviolette P,
    4. Philippe L,
    5. Coulombe D
    (1986) The curve-shift paradigm in self-stimulation. Physiol Behav 37:85–91.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Negus SS,
    2. Mello NK,
    3. Linsenmayer DC,
    4. Jones RM,
    5. Portoghese PS
    (2002) Kappa opioid antagonist effects of the novel κ-antagonist 5′-guanidinonaltrindole (GNTI) in an assay of schedule-controlled behavior in rhesus monkeys. Psychopharmacology 163:412–419.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Nestler EJ,
    2. Barrot M,
    3. DiLeone RJ,
    4. Eisch AJ,
    5. Gold SJ,
    6. Monteggia LM
    (2002) Neurobiology of depression. Neuron 34:13–25.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Newton SS,
    2. Thome J,
    3. Wallace TL,
    4. Shirayama Y,
    5. Schlesinger L,
    6. Sakai N,
    7. Chen J,
    8. Neve R,
    9. Nestler EJ,
    10. Duman RS
    (2002) Inhibition of cAMP response element-binding protein or dynorphin in the nucleus accumbens produces an antidepressant-like effect. J Neurosci 22:10883–10890.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    1. Pasquier DA,
    2. Kemper TL,
    3. Forbes WB,
    4. Morgane PJ
    (1977) Dorsal raphe, substantia nigra and locus coeruleus: interconnections with each other and the neostriatum. Brain Res Bull 2:323–339.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Paulson PE,
    2. Camp DM,
    3. Robinson TE
    (1991) Time course of transient behavioral depression and persistent behavioral sensitization in relation to regional brain monoamine concentrations during amphetamine withdrawal in rats. Psychopharmacology 103:480–492.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Paxinos G,
    2. Watson C
    (1986) The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates (Academic Press, San Diego), 2nd ed..
  29. ↵
    1. Pfeiffer A,
    2. Brantl V,
    3. Herz A,
    4. Emrich HM
    (1986) Psychotomimesis mediated by κ-opiate receptors. Science (Wash DC) 233:774–776.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. ↵
    1. Pliakas AM,
    2. Carlson R,
    3. Neve RL,
    4. Konradi C,
    5. Nestler EJ,
    6. Carlezon WA, Jr
    (2001) Altered responsiveness to cocaine and increased immobility in the forced swim test associated with elevated cAMP response element binding protein expression in nucleus accumbens. J Neurosci 21:7397–7403.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. ↵
    1. Porsolt RD,
    2. Le Pichon M,
    3. Jalfre M
    (1977) Depression: a new animal model sensitive to antidepressant treatments. Nature (Lond) 266:730–732.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. Post RM,
    2. Kotin J,
    3. Goodwin FK
    (1974) The effects of cocaine on depressed patients. Am J Psychiatry 131:511–516.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  33. ↵
    1. Ritz MC,
    2. Kuhar MJ
    (1989) Relationship between self-administration of amphetamine and monoamine receptors in brain: comparison with cocaine. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 248:1010–1017.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  34. ↵
    1. Shippenberg TS,
    2. Rea W
    (1997) Sensitization to the behavioral effects of cocaine: modulation by dynorphin and κ-opioid receptor agonists. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 57:449–455.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Spanagel R,
    2. Shippenberg TS
    (1993) Modulation of morphine-induced sensitization by endogenous κ-opioid systems in the rat. Neurosci Lett 153:232–236.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Stevens WC, Jr,
    2. Jones RM,
    3. Subramanian G,
    4. Metzger TG,
    5. Ferguson DM,
    6. Portoghese PS
    (2000) Potent and selective indolomorphinan antagonists of the κ-opioid receptor. J Med Chem 43:2759–2769.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Svingos AL,
    2. Colago EE,
    3. Pickel VM
    (1999) Cellular sites for dynorphin activation of κ-opioid receptors in the rat nucleus accumbens shell. J Neurosci 19:1804–1813.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  38. ↵
    1. Wise RA
    (1982) Neuroleptics and operant behavior: The anhedonia hypothesis. Behav Brain Sci 5:39–87.
  39. ↵
    1. Wise RA
    (1998) Drug-activation of brain reward pathways. Drug Alcohol Depend 51:13–22.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Zhang XF,
    2. Hu XT,
    3. White FJ
    (1998) Whole-cell plasticity in cocaine withdrawal: reduced sodium currents in nucleus accumbens neurons. J Neurosci 18:488–498.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
View Abstract
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics: 305 (1)
Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
Vol. 305, Issue 1
1 Apr 2003
  • Table of Contents
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
Download PDF
Article Alerts
Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Email Article

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Antidepressant-Like Effects of κ-Opioid Receptor Antagonists in the Forced Swim Test in Rats
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Research ArticleBEHAVIORAL PHARMACOLOGY

Antidepressant-Like Effects of κ-Opioid Receptor Antagonists in the Forced Swim Test in Rats

Stephen D. Mague, Andrea M. Pliakas, Mark S. Todtenkopf, Hilarie C. Tomasiewicz, Yan Zhang, William C. Stevens, Robert M. Jones, Philip S. Portoghese and William A. Carlezon
Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics April 1, 2003, 305 (1) 323-330; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1124/jpet.102.046433

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Share
Research ArticleBEHAVIORAL PHARMACOLOGY

Antidepressant-Like Effects of κ-Opioid Receptor Antagonists in the Forced Swim Test in Rats

Stephen D. Mague, Andrea M. Pliakas, Mark S. Todtenkopf, Hilarie C. Tomasiewicz, Yan Zhang, William C. Stevens, Robert M. Jones, Philip S. Portoghese and William A. Carlezon
Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics April 1, 2003, 305 (1) 323-330; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1124/jpet.102.046433
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Concentration-response analysis human taste discrimination
  • Varenicline and epibatidine as opioid adjuvants
  • MCAM reverses and prevents fentanyl ventilatory depression
Show more Behavioral Pharmacology

Similar Articles

  • Home
  • Alerts
Facebook   Twitter   LinkedIn   RSS

Navigate

  • Current Issue
  • Fast Forward by date
  • Fast Forward by section
  • Latest Articles
  • Archive
  • Search for Articles
  • Feedback
  • ASPET

More Information

  • About JPET
  • Editorial Board
  • Instructions to Authors
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Customized Alerts
  • RSS Feeds
  • Subscriptions
  • Permissions
  • Terms & Conditions of Use

ASPET's Other Journals

  • Drug Metabolism and Disposition
  • Molecular Pharmacology
  • Pharmacological Reviews
  • Pharmacology Research & Perspectives
ISSN 1521-0103 (Online)

Copyright © 2021 by the American Society for Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics