Skip to main content
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current Issue
    • Fast Forward
    • Latest Articles
    • Archive
  • Information
    • Instructions to Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • FAQs
    • For Subscribers
    • Terms & Conditions of Use
    • Permissions
  • Editorial Board
  • Alerts
    • Alerts
    • RSS Feeds
  • Virtual Issues
  • Feedback
  • Other Publications
    • Drug Metabolism and Disposition
    • Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
    • Molecular Pharmacology
    • Pharmacological Reviews
    • Pharmacology Research & Perspectives
    • ASPET

User menu

  • My alerts
  • Log in
  • Log out
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
  • Other Publications
    • Drug Metabolism and Disposition
    • Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
    • Molecular Pharmacology
    • Pharmacological Reviews
    • Pharmacology Research & Perspectives
    • ASPET
  • My alerts
  • Log in
  • Log out
  • My Cart
Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current Issue
    • Fast Forward
    • Latest Articles
    • Archive
  • Information
    • Instructions to Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • FAQs
    • For Subscribers
    • Terms & Conditions of Use
    • Permissions
  • Editorial Board
  • Alerts
    • Alerts
    • RSS Feeds
  • Virtual Issues
  • Feedback
  • Visit jpet on Facebook
  • Follow jpet on Twitter
  • Follow jpet on LinkedIn
OtherANALGESIA AND DRUGS OF ABUSE

Differences in the Antinociceptive Effects ofAlpha-2 Adrenoceptor Agonists in Two Substrains of Sprague-Dawley Rats

Brent A. Graham, Donna L. Hammond and Herbert K. Proudfit
Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics November 1997, 283 (2) 511-519;
Brent A. Graham
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Donna L. Hammond
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Herbert K. Proudfit
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

In this study, we examined whether Sprague-Dawley rats obtained from two different vendors, Harlan and Sasco, differ with respect to the types of alpha-2 adrenoceptors in the spinal cord that mediate antinociception. This hypothesis was tested using twoalpha-2 adrenoceptor agonists, dexmedetomidine and ST-91, which are relatively selective for alpha-2A andalpha-2B adrenoceptors, respectively, and two different measures of nociception, the tail-flick and the 55°C hot-plate test. Dexmedetomidine and ST-91 each increased tail-flick latency to a similar extent in both Harlan and Sasco rats, although dexmedetomidine was more efficacious than ST-91 in each substrain. However, the efficacy of these agonists was markedly different in Harlan and Sasco rats when the hot-plate test was used. For example, ST-91 was a full agonist in the hot-plate test in Harlan rats but a weak partial agonist in Sasco rats. Dexmedetomidine was a very weak partial agonist in Harlan rats and ineffective in the hot-plate test in Sasco rats. These findings suggest that (1) both spinal alpha-2A andalpha-2B receptors modulate nociceptive responses in the tail-flick test in both Harlan and Sasco rats; (2) hot-plate responses are mediated predominantly by alpha-2B adrenoceptors, with a minimal contribution by alpha-2A adrenoceptors in the Harlan rat and (3) hot-plate responses are not appreciably affected by either alpha-2A or alpha-2B adrenoceptors in the Sasco rat. These findings confirm previous reports that intrathecal administration of alpha-2 adrenoceptor agonists produces thermal antinociception in the rat. However, the magnitude of the antinociceptive effect is dependent on the receptor selectivity of the agonist used, cutaneous tissue stimulated to elicit nociceptive responses and substrain of rat.

It is well established that i.t. administration of alpha-2 adrenoceptor agonists such as dexmedetomidine (Fisher et al., 1991; Idänpään-Heikkilä et al., 1994; Kalso et al., 1991; Takano and Yaksh, 1992), clonidine (Ossipov et al., 1988; Reddy et al., 1980; Solomon et al., 1989; Takano and Yaksh, 1992) or ST-91 (Howe et al., 1983; Monasky et al., 1990; Saeki and Yaksh, 1992; Takano and Yaksh, 1992) produces antinociception in rats (Kalso et al., 1991; Reddy et al., 1980;Takano and Yaksh, 1992), monkeys (Yaksh and Reddy, 1981) and humans (Filos et al., 1994; Gordh, 1988; Gordh and Tamsen, 1983;Tamsen and Gordh, 1984). Subsequent studies with idazoxan, yohimbine, prazosin and imiloxan indicated that these alpha-2 adrenoceptor antagonists differ in their rank order of potency to attenuate the antinociception produced by dexmedetomidine, clonidine and ST-91 in the Harlan Sprague-Dawley rat (Takano et al., 1992; Takano and Yaksh, 1992). Based on these differences, Yaksh and colleagues proposed that dexmedetomidine and clonidine produce antinociception by acting at alpha-2A adrenoceptors, whereas ST-91 acts at alpha-2B adrenoceptors (Takano et al., 1992; Takano and Yaksh, 1992).

The alpha-2 adrenoceptor agonists produce antinociception by inhibiting synaptic transmission in the rat spinal cord dorsal horn (Kalso et al., 1993; Murata et al., 1989;Sullivan et al., 1987, 1992). Stimulation of noradrenergic neurons in the rat brainstem also produces antinociception (Jones, 1992; Proudfit, 1992; Yeomans et al., 1992; West et al., 1993) by inhibiting the responses of dorsal horn neurons to noxious stimuli (Jones and Gebhart, 1986, 1987). Spinally projecting neurons in the A5, A6 (locus ceruleus) and A7 noradrenergic cell groups located in the pons are the predominant source of norepinephrine in the spinal cord of the rat (Nygren and Olson, 1976; Westlund et al., 1982, 1983). However, recent studies using more sensitive tract tracing and immunocytochemical methods have determined that the origin of the catecholaminergic projection to the dorsal horn differs significantly in Sprague-Dawley rats obtained from two different vendors. In Sprague-Dawley rats obtained from Harlan, locus ceruleus neurons project predominantly to the superficial laminae of the dorsal horn (Clark and Proudfit, 1992; Fritschy and Grzanna, 1990; Grzanna and Fritschy, 1991), whereas the axons of A7 neurons terminate in the ventral horn (Lyons et al., 1989). In contrast, in Sprague-Dawley rats obtained from Sasco, A7 neurons project mainly to laminae I–IV in the ipsilateral dorsal horn (Clark and Proudfit, 1991b), whereas locus ceruleus neurons project most heavily to the ipsilateral medial aspect of laminae VII–IX, with only a minor projection to the ventral part of the dorsal horn (Clark and Proudfit, 1991a; Proudfit and Clark, 1991). This substrain difference between Harlan and Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats has been replicated by other investigators (Sluka and Westlund, 1992). Initial studies of the ability of intrathecally administered alpha-2 adrenoceptor antagonists to attenuate the antinociception produced by electrical (West et al., 1993; Yeomans et al., 1992) or chemical (Yeomans and Proudfit, 1992) stimulation demonstrated significant functional differences between the catecholamine cell groups in these substrains. These results raised the possibility that the alpha-2 adrenoceptor subtypes that mediate antinociception in the spinal cord of these substrains may also be different.

The present experiments were done as part of a systematic study of the spinal cord alpha-2 adrenoceptors that modulate nociception in Sprague-Dawley rats obtained from Harlan and Sasco (Graham et al., 1995a, 1995b, 1996, 1997). In this aspect of the study, we report the effects of the alpha-2 adrenoceptor agonists dexmedetomidine and ST-91 on nociceptive responses determined using the tail-flick and 55°C hot-plate tests in both Harlan and Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats.

Methods

Surgical preparation.

Male Sprague-Dawley rats (250–350 g) obtained from Harlan (Indianapolis, IN) and Sasco (Madison, WI) were anesthetized with halothane and implanted with i.t. catheters that terminated at the L4 segment of the spinal cord (Hammond, 1988; Yaksh and Rudy, 1976). The rats were allowed to recover for 10 days before testing and those exhibiting neurological deficits such as hemiparesis of fore paws or hindpaws were killed. Rats were used only once in this study, after which they were killed and the location and patency of each catheter were determined by direct visualization after an injection of India ink.

Behavioral testing.

Thermal nociceptive thresholds were measured using the tail-flick (D’Amour and Smith, 1941) and hot-plate (Woolfe and MacDonald, 1944) tests. In the tail-flick test, a high-intensity light beam was focused on the dorsal surface of the rat’s blackened tail. The reflex removal of the tail from the light beam was electronically measured to the nearest 0.1 sec, and the average of two successive measurements was defined as the TFL. In the hot-plate test, the rat was placed on an enclosed 55°C copper surface, and the latency to a hindpaw lick or a jump off the surface was measured. One determination of the hot-plate response latency, measured to the nearest 0.1 sec, was made and defined as the HPL.

Drug-induced motor dysfunction can alter measurements of TFL or HPL and confound the interpretation of drug effects on nociception (Hammond, 1989). Motor function was therefore assessed by the inclined plane and righting reflex tests (Rivlin and Tator, 1977). To test the righting reflex, the rat was placed on its back, allowed to right itself and assigned a score of 0 to 2 based on execution of the reflex (Hammond and Washington, 1993). In the inclined plane test, the rat was placed perpendicular to the axis of an inclined plane having a rubber surface with horizontal ridges 0.1 cm in height. The angle of the inclined plane was adjusted in 5° increments to determine the largest angle at which the rat was able to maintain its position on the incline. Doses of drugs that produced significant motor deficits, as measured by the righting reflex and the inclined plane, were not included in the analysis of dose-response functions.

Drugs.

The drugs tested were the relatively selectivealpha-2 adrenoceptor agonists dexmedetomidine (molecular weight = 236.7; Orion Corp., Orion-Farmos, Turku, Finland) and ST-91 (molecular weight = 253.8; Boehringer Ingelheim, Ridgefield, CT). Dexmedetomidine was injected in doses ranging from 1.3 to 12.7 nmol, and ST-91 was injected in doses ranging from 3.9 to 39.4 nmol. All drugs were dissolved in normal saline; pH of each solution was adjusted to 7.0 and administered intrathecally in a volume of 10 μl, followed by 10 μl of normal saline to flush the catheter. The injection volume was monitored by following movement of an air bubble through a calibrated length of tubing.

Experimental design.

On the day before testing, the rats were brought to the testing environment from the animal care facility, their tails were blackened and they were allowed to acclimate to the testing room for several hours. The following day, they were returned to the testing environment and allowed to acclimate for ≥1 hour. Rats with mean baseline TFL values of >5.2 sec or baseline HPL values of >15 sec or those in which the inclined plane angle was <40° were excluded from further testing. Dexmedetomidine, ST-91, a mixture of dexmedetomidine and ST-91 or a saline control solution was then injected i.t., and response latencies were redetermined 10, 20, 30, 45 and 60 min later. Motor function was reassessed using the inclined plane and righting reflex tests at 30 min, which corresponds to the time of peak antinociceptive effect. Animals that did not respond by 14 sec in the tail-flick test or 40 sec in the hot-plate test after drug administration were assigned that cutoff latency. The tests were conducted in tandem, with the tail-flick test conducted before the hot-plate test. These experiments were conducted under a protocol approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Chicago and in accordance with the “Guide for Care and Use of Laboratory Animals” of the National Institutes of Health.

Statistical analysis.

The effect of each drug dose was compared with that of saline using a two-way analysis of variance for repeated measures in which one factor was drug treatment and the repeated factor was time. Multiple post hoc comparisons among individual mean values were made using the Newman-Keuls test (Keppel, 1973). ED50 values for each agonist were determined from dose-response curves generated by least-squares linear regression of response latency values at the time of peak effect from individual animals. Fieller’s theorem was used to determine 95% CLs (Finney, 1964). The ED50 was defined as the dose that produced one half of the maximal possible increase in response latency; this value was 9 sec for the tail-flick test and 25 sec for the hot-plate test. Dose-response curves were compared for parallelism and for differences in ED50 values by analysis of covariance (Zar, 1974). A value of P ≤ .05 was considered to be statistically significant.

Results

Effects of intrathecally administered dexmedetomidine on TFL in Sasco and Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats.

Intrathecal administration of dexmedetomidine produced a dose-dependent increase in TFL in both Sasco Sprague-Dawley (figs. 1A and2A) and Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats (figs.1B and 2A). This effect was apparent within 10 min, and the peak effect was achieved by 20 min (fig. 1). The increase in TFL persisted for ≥60 min for all drug doses. The ED50 (95% CL) of dexmedetomidine was 4.3 (2.9–6.0) nmol in Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats and 3.4 (1.9–5.3) nmol in Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats. The difference between these values was not statistically significant (fig. 2A; P > .05).

Figure 1
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1

Time course of the increases in TFL produced by i.t. administration of saline (○) or 1.3 (□), 4.2 (▪), 8.4 (•) or 12.7 (♦) nmol of dexmedetomidine in Sasco Sprague-Dawley (A) and Harlan Sprague-Dawley (B) rats. Each value is the mean ± S.E.M. TFL value of determinations in 7 to 16 rats. ∗P < .05, ∗∗P < .01, values significantly different from those in saline-treated rats at the corresponding time point.

Figure 2
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2

Dose-response relationships for the antinociceptive effect of dexmedetomidine administered by the i.t. route in Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats (•) and Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats (▪) in the tail-flick test (A) and the hot-plate test (B). Mean response latency values in saline-treated rats (S) are represented by the open symbols. Each value is the mean ± S.E.M. of determinations in 7 to 16 animals. Significant regressions are represented by the lines of best fit, which were determined by least-squares linear regression of the individual response latency values obtained 20 min after agonist drug administration, which corresponded to the time of peak effect.

Effects of intrathecally administered dexmedetomidine on HPL in Sasco and Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats.

Unlike the tail-flick test, i.t. administration of dexmedetomidine to Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats in doses as high as 12.7 nmol did not significantly increase response latencies in the hot-plate test compared with values in saline-treated rats (fig. 3A). The administration of a higher dose of 42.2 nmol produced profound sedation and prostration, which interfered with the measurement of response latencies on the hot-plate test. In contrast, i.t. administration of 8.4 or 12.7 nmol of dexmedetomidine to Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats produced a modest increase in HPL that occurred within 20 min and persisted for ≥60 min. Although a dose of 4.2 nmol of dexmedetomidine also increased HPL, this effect was not apparent until 45 min after drug administration (fig.3B). No dose of dexmedetomidine increased HPL beyond the criterion value of 25 sec in either Sasco or Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats, which precluded the calculation of an ED50 value for dexmedetomidine in this test (fig. 2B).

Figure 3
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3

Time course of the increases in HPL produced by i.t. administration of saline (○) or 1.3 (□), 4.2 (▪), 8.4 (•) or 12.7 (♦) nmol of dexmedetomidine in Sasco Sprague-Dawley (A) and Harlan Sprague-Dawley (B) rats. Each value is the mean ± S.E.M. HPL value of determinations in 7 to 16 animals. ∗P < .05, ∗∗P < .01, values significantly different from those in saline-treated rats at the corresponding time point.

Effects of intrathecally administered ST-91 on TFL in Sasco and Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats.

Intrathecal administration of ST-91 produced a dose-dependent increase in TFL in both Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats (fig. 4A) and Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats (fig. 4B). Mean TFL values were significantly increased within 20 min, reached peak values by 30 min and remained elevated for at least 60 min (fig. 4, A and B). ST-91 appeared to be a partial agonist in the tail-flick test in Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats (figs. 4A and5A), as well as in Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats (figs. 4B and 5A). Thus, mean TFL values increased to ∼9 to 10 sec after i.t. administration of 11.8 nmol of ST-91, but no further increase in TFL occurred after i.t. administration of 39.4 nmol, a 3-fold higher dose, in either substrain (figs. 4, A and B, and 5A). The administration of ST-91 in a higher dose of 118.2 nmol produced spontaneous serpentine tail movements in Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats that prevented accurate measurements of TFL. Because ST-91 did not increase TFL beyond the criterion value of 9 sec, it was not possible to determine an ED50 value and 95% CL for this agonist in either substrain (fig. 5A).

Figure 4
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4

Time course of the increases in TFL produced by i.t. administration of saline (○) or 3.9 (▪), 11.8 (•) or 39.4 (♦) nmol of ST-91 in Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats (A) and Harlan Sprague-Dawley (B) rats. Each value is the mean ± S.E.M. TFL value of determinations in 6 to 15 animals. ∗P < .05, ∗∗P < .01, values that are significantly different from those in saline-treated rats at the corresponding time point.

Figure 5
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5

Dose-response relationships for the antinociceptive effects of ST-91 administered by the i.t. route in Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats (•) and Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats (▪) in the tail-flick test (A) and the hot-plate test (B). Mean response latency values in saline-treated rats (S) are represented by the open symbols. Each value is the mean ± S.E.M. of determinations in 6 to 15 animals. Significant regressions are represented by the lines of best fit determined by least-squares linear regression of individual response latency values obtained 30 min after agonist drug administration, which corresponded to the time of peak effect.

Effects of intrathecally administered ST-91 on HPL in Sasco and Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats.

The i.t. administration of 11.8 or 39.4 nmol of ST-91 to Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats increased mean HPL to ∼20 sec within 30 min of drug injection (fig.6A). The antinociceptive effect of a higher dose of ST-91 could not be determined due to the presence of spontaneous motor effects as noted above. No dose of ST-91 increased HPL beyond the criterion value of 25 sec in Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats, which precluded the calculation of an ED50 value (fig. 5B).

Figure 6
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6

Time course of the increases in HPL produced by i.t. administration of saline (○) or 3.9 (▪), 11.8 (•) or 39.4 (♦) nmol of ST-91 in Sasco Sprague-Dawley (A) and Harlan Sprague-Dawley (B) rats. Each value is the mean ± S.E.M. HPL value of determinations in 6 to 15 animals. ∗P < .05, ∗∗P < .01, values that are significantly different from those in saline-treated rats at the corresponding time point.

In contrast, i.t. injection of ST-91 in Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats in a dose of 11.8 or 39.4 nmol increased HPL in a dose-dependent manner, with the peak effect observed at 30 min (fig. 6B). ST-91 injected in the lowest dose of 3.9 nmol also increased HPL, but this effect achieved statistical significance only at the 20-min time point. The ED50 value (and 95% CL) of ST-91 in Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats was 15.5 (8.2–35.3) nmol (fig. 5B).

Effects of combined i.t. administration of ST-91 and dexmedetomidine on TFL in Sasco and Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats.

In both Harlan and Sasco rats, dexmedetomidine was a full agonist, whereas ST-91 was a partial agonist in the tail-flick test. If these agonists act at the same receptor, then coadministration of the low-efficacy agonist ST-91 with the high-efficacy agonist dexmedetomidine should produce an antinociceptive effect that is less than that produced by dexmedetomidine alone. However, coadministration of ST-91 did not attenuate the antinociceptive effects of dexmedetomidine but rather produced a greater increase in TFL than dexmedetomidine alone at several time points in both Sasco and Harlan rats (P < .05; fig.7, A and B).

Figure 7
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7

Time course of the increases in TFL produced by i.t. administration of saline (○), 39.4 nmol of ST-91 (♦), 12.7 nmol of dexmedetomidine (▴) or a mixture of 39.4 nmol of ST-91 and 12.7 nmol of dexmedetomidine (▪) in Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats (A) and Harlan Sprague-Dawley (B) rats. The TFL values for saline, ST-91 and dexmedetomidine were taken from figures 1 and 4. Each value is the mean ± S.E.M. TFL value of determinations in 6 to 15 animals. ∗P < .05, ∗∗P < .01, values that are significantly greater than those in dexmedetomidine-treated rats at the corresponding time point.

Effects of combined i.t. administration of ST-91 and dexmedetomidine on HPL in Sasco and Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats.

In Sasco rats, dexmedetomidine was ineffective and ST-91 produced only a modest increase in HPL (fig. 8A). Coadministration of the low-efficacy agonist dexmedetomidine with the high-efficacy agonist ST-91 increased HPL to nearly 40 sec in this substrain (fig. 8A). In Harlan rats, dexmedetomidine produced a modest increase in HPL, whereas ST-91 produced a much greater increase in HPL (fig. 8B). The coadministration of dexmedetomidine with ST-91 in this substrain also produced a large increase in HPL that was significantly greater than that of ST-91 alone (fig. 8B).

Figure 8
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 8

Time course of the increases in HPL produced by i.t. administration of saline (○), 39.4 nmol of ST-91 (♦), 12.7 nmol of dexmedetomidine (▴) or a mixture of 39.4 nmol of ST-91 and 12.7 nmol of dexmedetomidine (▪) in Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats (A) and Harlan Sprague-Dawley (B) rats. The HPL values for saline, ST-91 and dexmedetomidine were taken from figures 3 and 5. Each value is the mean ± S.E.M. HPL value of determinations in 6 to 15 animals. ∗P < .05, ∗∗P < .01, values that are significantly greater than those in ST-91-treated rats at the corresponding time point

Discussion

The antinociceptive effects of dexmedetomidine and ST-91 differ in Harlan and Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats. The present study provides evidence that Harlan Sprague-Dawley and Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats differ with respect to the types of alpha-2 adrenoceptor in the spinal cord that mediate thermal antinociception. If the spinalalpha-2 adrenoceptors that mediate antinociception in each substrain are the same, then the antinociceptive effects of dexmedetomidine should be the same in each substrain. Similarly, the antinociceptive effects of ST-91 an alpha-2B adrenoceptor agonist should be the same in each substrain. This was not the case in the present study. The most marked difference between the substrains was observed in the hot-plate test. Dexmedetomidine increased HPL to 18 to 20 sec in Harlan rats but was ineffective in Sasco rats (table1 and fig. 2). ST-91 increased HPL to 30 sec in Harlan rats but was much less effective in Sasco rats (table 1and fig. 5). These differences suggest that the neural circuitry that subserves responses in the hot-plate test in Harlan rats differs from Sasco rats in (1) the number or distribution of alpha-2 adrenoceptors in the dorsal horn, (2) the relative densities of the different subtypes of alpha-2 adrenoceptor or (3) the efficiency of the coupling of alpha-2 adrenoceptors to subcellular effectors such as G proteins (Summers and McMartin, 1993). In contrast to the hot-plate test, there were no significant differences between the two substrains in the antinociceptive effects of either alpha-2 adrenoceptor agonist in the tail-flick test. Dexmedetomidine increased TFL with similar potency and to a maximal value of 12 to 13 sec in both Harlan and Sasco rats (table 1and fig. 2). ST-91 also increased TFL with similar potencies in both Harlan and Sasco rats, although to maximal values of only 10 sec (table1 and fig. 5). The similarity of the effects of dexmedetomidine and ST-91 in both substrains in the tail-flick test suggests that the distribution, density and types of alpha-2 adrenoceptors that mediate antinociception in the tail-flick test are similar in both Harlan and Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1

Summary of the antinociceptive effects of dexmedetomidine and ST-91: Comparison between Harlan and Sasco rats

The antinociceptive effects of dexmedetomidine and ST-91 result from actions at different alpha-2 adrenoceptors. Dexmedetomidine and ST-91 exhibited substantially different antinociceptive efficacies in the tail-flick and hot-plate tests in both substrains of rat. For example, in the tail-flick test dexmedetomidine was more potent and appeared to be a full agonist (fig. 2 and table2), whereas ST-91 was less potent and appeared to be a partial agonist in Harlan and Sasco rats (fig. 5 and table 2). Even larger differences in the efficacy of these twoalpha-2 adrenoceptor agonists were observed in the hot-plate test, although in this test ST-91 was more efficacious than dexmedetomidine in each substrain. There are three possible explanations for such differences in the apparent potency and efficacy of dexmedetomidine and ST-91 in Sasco and Harlan rats. First, dexmedetomidine and ST-91 could act at the same alpha-2 adrenoceptor in both substrains, one of which has feweralpha-2 adrenoceptors in the dorsal horn. Second, these twoalpha-2 adrenoceptor agonists could act at the samealpha-2 adrenoceptor in both substrains, one of which has a lower intrinsic activity due to less efficient receptor coupling to subcellular effectors. Third, dexmedetomidine and ST-91 could act at different alpha-2 adrenoceptor subtypes. The first two possibilities merit consideration because shifts in the dose-response curves for dexmedetomidine and ST-91 produced by the irreversible antagonist N-ethoxycarbonyl-2-ethoxy-1,2-dihydroquinoline (Takano and Yaksh, 1991) or by variation in the intensity of noxious stimuli (Saeki and Yaksh, 1992) suggest that dexmedetomidine has lower fractional receptor occupancy requirements and greater intrinsic activity than ST-91. To resolve these possibilities, dexmedetomidine and ST-91 were coadministered. The administration of a full agonist with a partial agonist that acts at the same receptor will produce an effect that is less than that produced by the full agonist alone because the partial agonist, which exhibits a lower efficacy, competes with the full agonist for available receptor binding sites (Kenakin, 1993). However, in both the tail-flick and hot-plate tests, coadministration of the twoalpha-2 adrenoceptor agonists consistently enhanced, rather than attenuated, the antinociceptive effect of the full agonist. This finding suggests that dexmedetomidine and ST-91 do not have different intrinsic activities at the same alpha-2 adrenoceptor but rather act at different subtypes of the alpha-2 adrenoceptor. This conclusion is consistent with the results of earlier studies that concluded that dexmedetomidine acts predominantly atalpha-2A receptors and ST-91 acts predominantly atalpha-2B receptors (Takano et al., 1992; Takano and Yaksh, 1992). It is also supported by our recent finding that coadministration of dexmedetomidine and ST-91 in a 1:3 fixed dose ratio increases TFL and HPL in Sasco and Harlan rats in a synergistic manner (Graham et al., 1997), as would be expected of two agonists that act at different receptors.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2

Comparison of the antinociceptive effects of dexmedetomidine and ST-91

The nociceptive responses of the feet and tail are modulated by different alpha-2 adrenoceptors. If the samealpha-2 adrenoceptors modulate responses in the tail-flick and hot-plate tests, then the rank order of potency of dexmedetomidine and ST-91 should be the same in both tests. This was not the case in the present study. For example, dexmedetomidine was a full agonist in the tail-flick test but a very weak partial agonist in Harlan rats and ineffective in Sasco rats in the hot-plate test. By comparison, ST-91 was a partial agonist in the tail-flick test, but in the hot-plate test it was a full agonist in Harlan rats and a weak partial agonist in Sasco rats. These differences in the relative efficacies of thealpha-2 adrenoceptor agonists on nociceptive responses of the feet, determined using the hot-plate test, compared with nociceptive responses of the tail suggest that differentalpha-2 adrenoceptors modulate nociceptive responses of the feet and tail.

The present results provide some insight into the relative importance of alpha-2 adrenoceptors in modulating the nociceptive responses of the feet and the tail. For example, both dexmedetomidine and ST-91 were effective in the tail-flick test in both Harlan and Sasco rats. Because dexmedetomidine and ST-91 act preferentially atalpha-2A and alpha-2B adrenoceptors, respectively, these findings suggest that both alpha-2A andalpha-2B adrenoceptors modulate nociceptive responses in the tail in both Sasco and Harlan rats. It cannot be stated at this time which subtype of alpha-2 adrenoceptor is of greater importance in modulating responses of the tail. Although ST-91 was not as efficacious as dexmedetomidine in the tail-flick test, it also has a higher fractional receptor occupancy and lower intrinsic activity than dexmedetomidine at alpha-2 adrenoceptors (Saeki and Yaksh, 1992; Takano and Yaksh, 1991). Clarification of the relative importance of alpha-2A and alpha-2B receptors in the tail-flick test must therefore await the identification ofalpha-2B adrenoceptor agonists with higher intrinsic activity than ST-91. Nevertheless, these observations suggest that the tail-flick test is a suitable measure for investigations of the potency or efficacy of alpha-2A or alpha-2B adrenoceptor ligands regardless of substrain.

In the hot-plate test, dexmedetomidine was a very weak partial agonist in Harlan rats and ineffective in Sasco rats, whereas ST-91 was a full agonist in Harlan rats and a weak partial agonist in Sasco rats. These findings suggest that in Harlan rats, responses in the hot-plate test are predominantly mediated by the alpha-2B adrenoceptor with a minimal contribution by alpha-2A receptors. In Sasco rats, the contribution of either alpha-2B or alpha-2A adrenoceptors appears to be minimal and much less than that in Harlan rats. These results suggest that the hot-plate test is not an appropriate measure for studies of the antinociceptive potency or efficacy of alpha-2A adrenoceptor ligands in either substrain. However, the hot-plate test is more suitable for studies of the antinociceptive effects of alpha-2B adrenoceptor agonists, particularly when determined using Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats.

These differences in the efficacy of alpha-2 adrenoceptor agonists in reducing nociceptive responses of the foot in the two substrains, as well as differences between the tail and the feet in each substrain, are relevant to the use of recently developed rodent models of persistent inflammatory pain and neuropathic pain (Bennett and Xie, 1988; Hargreaves et al., 1988; Kim and Chung, 1992). That is, the choice of substrain is likely to be a critical factor in establishing the therapeutic efficacy of different classes ofalpha-2 adrenoceptor agonists in these models in which the hindpaw, rather than the tail, is the site of stimulation.

Direct comparisons between the tail-flick and hot-plate test should be made cautiously because the tail-flick test involves a reflexive response and the hot-plate test involves a behaviorally integrated response. Moreover, the neural circuitry that subserves these responses is different. The tail-flick reflex is produced by activation of C-fiber afferents (Fleischer et al., 1983; Handwerkeret al., 1987; Necker and Hellon, 1978), which terminate in the S3-Co1 segments of the spinal cord (Grossman et al., 1982). By comparison, the primary afferents that innervate the plantar surface of the hindlimb travel predominantly in the tibial nerve to terminate in the L2–5 segments of the spinal cord (Molander and Grant, 1985, 1986). Activation of Aδ- as well as C-fiber afferents mediates the foot withdrawal response (Fleischer et al., 1983, Leemet al., 1993). Recent studies that used pseudorabies virus, a transneuronal tracer, to map the tail-flick reflex pathway indicated that there is one (and possibly more) interneuron in the tail-flick reflex pathway and that many of these are bilaterally distributed (Jasmin et al., 1997). Much less is known about the interneurons in the neural circuitry that subserves the hot-plate response; however, this pathway is also likely to be polysynaptic with a predominantly ipsilateral distribution of interneurons (Jasminet al., 1997; Rotto-Percelay et al., 1992). The observed differences in the effects of dexmedetomidine and ST-91 in the tail-flick and hot-plate tests could result from a differential distribution of alpha-2A and alpha-2B adrenoceptors in the neural circuitry that underlies nociceptive responses in the tail-flick and hot-plate tests. Segmental differences in the density and distribution of spinal alpha-2A andalpha-2B adrenoceptors could explain the different efficacies of dexmedetomidine and ST-91 in the tail-flick test compared with the hot-plate test in each substrain. However, the density ofalpha-2 binding sites identified by [3H]rauwolscine does not differ among the cervical, thoracic, lumbar and sacral segments of the spinal cord (Roudet et al., 1994). Similarly, differences between Harlan and Sasco rats in the efficacy of dexmedetomidine or ST-91 in the hot-plate test could reflect differences between these two substrains in the density and distribution of alpha-2A andalpha-2B adrenoceptors in the lumbar spinal cord. Quantitative autoradiographic studies of the segmental distribution ofalpha-2A and alpha-2B adrenoceptors in each substrain will be required to resolve these possibilities. Finally, the sites at which the alpha-2 adrenoceptor agonists may act to suppress nociceptive transmission are many and may include (1) terminals of the Aδ or C fiber primary afferents, (2) first- through last-order interneurons and (3) wide dynamic range or nocispecific spinothalamic, spinoreticular or spinomesencephalic neurons that comprise the afferent pain pathways of the tail and foot. Agonists for receptors that are present on multiple components of the afferent pain pathways could conceivably exhibit greater antinociceptive efficacy and potency than agonists for receptors that are preferentially expressed by only one component (e.g., only one class of primary afferent). Definitive identification of the sites at which dexmedetomidine and ST-91 act to suppress nociception will requirein situ hybridization studies or immunocytochemical studies that localize alpha-2A and alpha-2B adrenoceptors to immunocytochemically identified primary afferents, first- through last-order interneurons and projection neurons at the light and electron microscopic level.

Summary.

The results of this study suggest that the Harlan and Sasco substrains of Sprague-Dawley rat differ with respect to the subtypes of alpha-2 adrenoceptors that mediate antinociception. This work complements previous investigations that identified differences between these two substrains in the origin of the noradrenergic innervation of the spinal cord dorsal horn. The results of this study suggest that both spinal alpha-2A andalpha-2B receptors modulate responses in the tail-flick test in both Harlan and Sasco rats and that this noradrenergic mechanism appears to be “conserved” between substrains. The tail-flick test is therefore a suitable model for examining the actions ofalpha-2 adrenoceptor agonists and antagonists in spinal cord. These two substrains do differ importantly with respect to the spinal alpha-2 adrenoceptor subtypes that modulate responses in the hot-plate test. Specifically, responses in the hot-plate test in Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats are mediated predominantly byalpha-2B adrenoceptors with a minimal contribution byalpha-2A adrenoceptors. In contrast, responses in the hot-plate test in Sasco Sprague-Dawley rats do not appear to be mediated appreciably by either alpha-2A oralpha-2B adrenoceptors. These findings confirm previous reports that intrathecally administered alpha-2 adrenoceptor agonists produce antinociception in models of thermal nociception in the rat. However, the magnitude of the antinociceptive effect depends on the receptor selectivity of the agonist used, cutaneous tissue stimulated to elicit nociceptive responses and substrain of rat.

Acknowledgments

The excellent technical assistance of Henry Z. Pitzele is appreciated.

Footnotes

  • Send reprint requests to: Brent A. Graham, M.D., Department of Anesthesia and Critical Care, University of Chicago, 5841 S. Maryland Avenue, M/C 4028, Chicago, IL 60637. E-mail:bagraham{at}midway.uchicago.edu

  • ↵1 This work was supported in part by United States Public Health Service Grant DA03980 (H.K.P.) and a Research Starter Grant from the Foundation for Anesthesia Education and Research and Hoeschst Marion Roussel (B.A.G.).

  • Abbreviations:
    i.t.
    intrathecal
    CL
    confidence limit
    HPL
    hot-plate latency
    ST-91
    2-(2,6-diethylphenylamino)-2-imidazoline
    TFL
    tail-flick latency
    • Received November 11, 1996.
    • Accepted July 15, 1997.
  • The American Society for Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics

References

  1. ↵
    1. Bennett G. J.,
    2. Xie Y.-K.
    (1988) A peripheral mononeuropathy in rat that produces disorders of pain sensation like those seen in man. Pain 33:87–107.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Clark F. M.,
    2. Proudfit H. K.
    (1991a) The projection of locus coeruleus neurons to the spinal cord in the rat determined by anterograde tracing combined with immunocytochemistry. Brain Res. 538:231–245.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Clark F. M.,
    2. Proudfit H. K.
    (1991b) The projection of noradrenergic neurons in the A7 catecholamine cell group to the spinal cord in the rat demonstrated by anterograde tracing combined with immunocytochemistry. Brain Res. 547:279–288.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Clark F. M.,
    2. Proudfit H. K.
    (1992) Anatomical evidence for genetic differences in the innervation of the rat spinal cord by noradrenergic locus coeruleus neurons. Brain Res. 591:44–53.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. D’Amour F. E.,
    2. Smith D. L.
    (1941) A method for determining loss of pain sensation. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 72:74–79.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  6. ↵
    1. Filos K. S.,
    2. Goudas L. C.,
    3. Patroni O.,
    4. Polyzou V.
    (1994) Hemodynamic and analgesic profile after intrathecal clonidine in humans. Anesthesiology 81:591–601.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Finney D. J.
    Statistical Method in Biological Assay (Hafner Publishing Co. New York).
  8. ↵
    1. Fisher B.,
    2. Zornow M. H.,
    3. Yaksh T. L.,
    4. Peterson B. M.
    (1991) Antinociceptive properties of intrathecal dexmedetomidine in rats. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 192:221–225.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Fleischer E.,
    2. Handwerker H. O.,
    3. Joukhadar S.
    (1983) Unmyelinated nociceptive units in two skin areas of the rat. Brain Res. 267:81–92.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Fritschy J. M.,
    2. Grzanna R.
    (1990) Demonstration of two separate descending noradrenergic pathways to the rat spinal cord: Evidence for an intragriseal trajectory of locus coeruleus axons in the superficial layers of the dorsal horn. J. Comp. Neurol. 291:553–582.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Gordh T.
    (1988) Epidural clonidine for treatment of post-operative pain after thoracotomy: A double-blind placebo-controlled study. Acta Anaesthesiol. Scand. 32:702–709.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  12. ↵
    Gordh, T. and Tamsen, A.: A study on the analgesic effect of clonidine in man. Acta Anaesthesiol. Scand. 87: suppl, 72, 1983..
  13. ↵
    1. Graham B. A.,
    2. Hammond D. L.,
    3. Proudfit H. K.
    (1995a) Spinal alpha2-noradrenergic receptors differ in substrains of Sprague-Dawley (S-D) rat. Neurosci. Abstr. 21:651.
    OpenUrl
  14. ↵
    1. Graham B. A.,
    2. Hammond D. L.,
    3. Proudfit H. K.
    (1996) Differential antinociceptive effects of α-2 adrenoceptor agonists in substrains of Sprague-Dawley rats (Abstract). Anesthesiology 85:A665.
    OpenUrl
  15. Graham, B. A., Hammond, D. L. and Proudfit, H. K.: Intrathecal dexmedetomidine (DEX) and ST-91 act synergistically to produce antinociception in the rat. Neurosci. Abstr.23: 1997..
  16. ↵
    1. Graham B. A.,
    2. Proudfit H. K.,
    3. Hammond D. L.
    (1995b) Different adrenoceptor subtypes mediate the antinociceptive effects of 2 agonists in different substrains of Sprague-Dawley rats (Abstract). Anesth. Analg. 80:S155.
    OpenUrl
  17. ↵
    1. Grossman M. L.,
    2. Basbaum A. I.,
    3. Fields H. L.
    (1982) Afferent and efferent connections in the rat tail-flick reflex (a model used to analyze pain control mechanisms). J. Comp. Neurol. 206:9–16.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Grzanna R.,
    2. Fritschy J.-M.
    (1991) Efferent projections of different subpopulations of central noradrenaline neurons. Prog. Brain Res. 88:89–101.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Hammond D. L.
    (1988) Intrathecal administration: Methodological considerations. Prog. Brain Res. 77:313–320.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Chapman C. R.,
    2. Loeser J. D.
    1. Hammond D. L.
    Inference of pain and its modulation from simple behaviors. in Issues in Pain Measurement, eds Chapman C. R., Loeser J. D. (Raven, New York), pp 69–91.
  21. ↵
    1. Hammond D. L.,
    2. Washington J. D.
    (1993) Antagonism of L-baclofen-induced antinociception by CGP 35348 in the spinal cord of the rat. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 234:255–262.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Handwerker H. O.,
    2. Anton F.,
    3. Reeh P. W.
    (1987) Discharge patterns of afferent cutaneous nerve fibers from the rat’s tail during prolonged noxious mechanical stimulation. Exp. Brain Res. 65:493–504.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Hargreaves K.,
    2. Dubner R.,
    3. Brown F.,
    4. Flores C.,
    5. Jores J.
    (1988) A new and sensitive method for measuring thermal nociception in cutaneous hyperalgesia. Pain 32:77–88.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Howe J. R.,
    2. Wang J.-Y.,
    3. Yaksh T. L.
    (1983) Selective antagonism of the antinociceptive effect of intrathecally applied alpha adrenergic agonists by intrathecal prazosin and intrathecal yohimbine. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 224:552–558.
  25. ↵
    1. Idänpään-Heikkilä J. J.,
    2. Kalso E. A.,
    3. Seppälä T.
    (1994) Antinociceptive actions of dexmedetomidine and the kappa-opioid agonist U-50,488H against noxious thermal, mechanical and inflammatory stimuli. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 271:1306–1313.
  26. ↵
    1. Jasmin L.,
    2. Carstens E.,
    3. Basbaum A. I.
    (1997) Interneurons presynaptic to rat tail-flick motoneurons as mapped by transneural transport of pseudorabies virus: Few have long ascending collaterals. Neuroscience 76:859–876.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Light A. R.
    1. Jones S. L.
    Descending control of nociception. in The Initial Processing of Pain and Its Descending Control: Spinal and Trigeminal Systems, ed Light A. R. (Karger, New York), 12, pp 203–295.
  28. ↵
    1. Jones S. L.,
    2. Gebhart G. F.
    (1986) Quantitative characterization of coeruleospinal inhibition of nociceptive transmission in the rat. J. Neurophysiol. 56:1397–1410.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  29. ↵
    1. Jones S. L.,
    2. Gebhart G. F.
    (1987) Spinal pathways mediating tonic, coeruleospinal, and raphe-spinal descending inhibition in the rat. J. Neurophysiol. 58:138–159.
  30. ↵
    1. Kalso E. A.,
    2. Pöyghiä R.,
    3. Rosenberg P. H.
    (1991) Spinal antinociception by dexmedetomidine, a highly selective alpha-2-adrenergic agonist. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 68:140–143.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Kalso E. A.,
    2. Sullivan A. F.,
    3. McQuay H. J.,
    4. Dickenson A. H.,
    5. Roques B. P.
    (1993) Cross-tolerance between mu opioid and alpha-2 adrenergic receptors, but not between mu and delta opioid receptors in the spinal cord of the rat. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 265:551–558.
  32. ↵
    1. Kenakin T.
    Pharmacologic Analysis of Drug-Receptor Interaction (Raven Press, New York).
  33. ↵
    1. Keppel G.
    Design and Analysis: A Researcher’s Handbook (Prentice-Hall, Inc. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey).
  34. ↵
    1. Kim S. H.,
    2. Chung J. M.
    (1992) An experimental model for peripheral neuropathy produced by segmental spinal nerve ligation in the rat. Pain 50:335–363.
  35. ↵
    1. Leem J. W.,
    2. Willis W. D.,
    3. Chung J. M.
    (1993) Cutaneous sensory receptors in the rat foot. J. Neurophysiol. 69:1684–1699.
  36. ↵
    1. Lyons W. E.,
    2. Fritschy J. M.,
    3. Grzanna R.
    (1989) The noradrenergic neurotoxin DSP-4 eliminates the coeruleospinal projection but spares projections of the A5 and A7 groups to the ventral horn of the rat spinal cord. J. Neurosci. 9:1481–1489.
  37. ↵
    1. Molander C.,
    2. Grant G.
    (1985) Cutaneous projections from the rat hindlimb foot to the substantia gelatinosa of the spinal cord studied by transganglionic transport of WGA-HRP conjugate. J. Comp. Neurol. 237:476–484.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Molander C.,
    2. Grant G.
    (1986) Laminar distribution and somatotopic organization of primary afferent fibers from hindlimb nerves in the dorsal horn: A study by transganglionic transport of horseradish peroxidase in the rat. Neuroscience 19:297–312.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Monasky M. S.,
    2. Zinsmeister A. R.,
    3. Stevens C. W.,
    4. Yaksh T. L.
    (1990) Interaction of intrathecal morphine and ST-91 on antinociception in the rat: Dose-response analysis, antagonism and clearance. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 254:383–392.
  40. ↵
    1. Murata K.,
    2. Nakagawa I.,
    3. Kumeta Y.,
    4. Kitahata L. M.,
    5. Collins J. G.
    (1989) Intrathecal clonidine suppresses noxiously evoked activity of spinal wide dynamic range neurons in cats. Anesth. Analg. 69:185–191.
  41. ↵
    1. Necker R.,
    2. Hellon R. F.
    (1978) Noxious thermal input from the rat tail: Modulation by descending inhibitory influences. Pain 4:231–242.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Nygren L. G.,
    2. Olson L.
    (1976) A new major projection from locus coeruleus: The main source of noradrenergic nerve terminals in the ventral and dorsal columns of the spinal cord. Brain Res. 132:85–93.
  43. ↵
    1. Ossipov M. H.,
    2. Suarez L. J.,
    3. Spaulding T. C.
    (1988) A comparison of the antinociceptive and behavioral effects of intrathecally administered opiates, α-2-adrenergic agonists, and local anesthetics in mice and rats. Anesth. Analg. 67:616–624.
  44. ↵
    1. Besson J.-M.
    1. Proudfit H. K.
    The behavioural pharmacology of the noradrenergic descending system. in Toward the Use of Noradrenergic Agonists for the Treatment of Pain, ed Besson J.-M. (Elsevier, Amsterdam), pp 119–136.
  45. ↵
    1. Proudfit H. K.,
    2. Clark F. M.
    (1991) The projections of locus coeruleus neurons to the spinal cord. Prog. Brain Res. 88:123–141.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Reddy S. V. R.,
    2. Maderdrut J. L.,
    3. Yaksh T. L.
    (1980) Spinal cord pharmacology of adrenergic agonist-mediated antinociception. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 213:525–533.
  47. ↵
    1. Rivlin A. S.,
    2. Tator C. H.
    (1977) Objective clinical assessment of motor function after experimental spinal cord injury in the rat. Neurosurgery 47:577–581.
    OpenUrl
  48. ↵
    1. Rotto-Percelay D. M.,
    2. Wheeler J. G.,
    3. Osorio F. A.,
    4. Platt K. B.,
    5. Loewy A. D.
    (1992) Transneuronal labeling of spinal interneurons and sympathetic preganglionic neurons after pseudorabies virus injections in the rat medial gastrocnemius muscle. Brain Res. 574:291–306.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Roudet C.,
    2. Mouchet P.,
    3. Feuerstein C.,
    4. Savasta M.
    (1994) Normal distribution of alpha 2-adrenoceptors in the rat spinal cord and its modification after noradrenergic denervations: A quantitative autoradiographic study. J. Neurosci. Res. 39:319–329.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Saeki S.,
    2. Yaksh T. L.
    (1992) Suppression by spinal alpha-2 agonists of motor and autonomic responses evoked by low- and high-intensity thermal stimuli. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 260:795–802.
  51. ↵
    1. Sluka K. A.,
    2. Westlund K. N.
    (1992) Spinal projections of the locus coeruleus and the nucleus subcoeruleus in the Harlan and the Sasco Sprague-Dawley rat. Brain Res. 579:67–73.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Solomon R. E.,
    2. Brody M. J.,
    3. Gebhart G. F.
    (1989) Pharmacological characterization of alpha adrenoceptors involved in the antinociceptive and cardiovascular effects of intrathecally administered clonidine. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 251:27–38.
  53. ↵
    1. Sullivan A. F.,
    2. Dashwood M. R.,
    3. Dickenson A. H.
    (1987) α2-Adrenoceptor modulation of nociception in rat spinal cord: Location, effects and interactions with morphine. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 138:169–177.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Sullivan A. F.,
    2. Kalso E. A.,
    3. McQuay H. J.,
    4. Dickenson A. H.
    (1992) The antinociceptive actions of dexmedetomidine on dorsal horn neuronal responses in the anaesthetized rat. Eur J Pharmacol 215:127–133.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Summers R. J.,
    2. McMartin L. R.
    (1993) Adrenoceptors and their second messenger systems. J. Neurochem. 60:10–23.
  56. ↵
    1. Takano Y.,
    2. Takano M.,
    3. Yaksh T. L.
    (1992) The effect of intrathecally administered imiloxan and WB4101: Possible role of α2-adrenoceptor subtypes in the spinal cord. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 219:465–468.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Takano Y.,
    2. Yaksh T. L.
    (1991) Relative efficacy of spinal alpha-2 agonists, dexmedetomidine, clonidine and ST-91, determined in vivo by using N-ethoxycarbonyl-2-ethoxy-1,2-dihydroquinoline, an irreversible antagonist. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 258:438–446.
  58. ↵
    1. Takano Y.,
    2. Yaksh T. L.
    (1992) Characterization of the pharmacology of intrathecally administered alpha-2 agonists and antagonists in rats. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 261:764–772.
  59. ↵
    1. Tamsen A.,
    2. Gordh T.
    (1984) Epidural clonidine produces analgesia. Lancet 2:231–232.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. West W. L.,
    2. Yeomans D. C.,
    3. Proudfit H. K.
    (1993) The function of noradrenergic neurons in mediating antinociception induced by electrical stimulation of the locus coeruleus in two different sources of Sprague-Dawley rats. Brain Res. 626:127–135.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Westlund K. N.,
    2. Bowker R. M.,
    3. Ziegler M. G.,
    4. Coulter J. D.
    (1982) Descending noradrenergic projections and their spinal terminations. Prog. Brain Res. 57:219–238.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  62. ↵
    1. Westlund K. N.,
    2. Bowker R. M.,
    3. Ziegler M. G.,
    4. Coulter J. D.
    (1983) Noradrenergic projections to the spinal cord in the rat. Brain Res. 263:15–31.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. Woolfe G.,
    2. MacDonald A. D.
    (1944) The evaluation of the analgesic action of pethidine hydrochloride (Demerol). J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 80:300–307.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  64. ↵
    1. Yaksh T. L.,
    2. Reddy S. V. R.
    (1981) Studies in the primate on the analgetic effects associated with intrathecal actions of opiates, α-adrenergic agonists and baclofen. Anesthesiology 54:451–467.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Yaksh T. L.,
    2. Rudy T. A.
    (1976) Chronic catheterization of the spinal subarachnoid space. Physiol. Behav. 17:1031–1036.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  66. ↵
    1. Yeomans D. C.,
    2. Clark F. M.,
    3. Paice J. A.,
    4. Proudfit H. K.
    (1992) Antinociception induced by electrical stimulation of spinally-projecting noradrenergic neurons in the A7 catecholamine cell group of the rat. Pain 48:449–461.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. ↵
    1. Yeomans D. C.,
    2. Proudfit H. K.
    (1992) Antinociception induced by microinjection of substance P into the A7 catecholamine cell group in the rat. Neuroscience 49:681–691.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  68. ↵
    1. Zar J. H.
    Biostatistical Analysis (Prentice-Hall, Inc. Englewood Cliffs, NJ).
View Abstract
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
Vol. 283, Issue 2
1 Nov 1997
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Download PDF
Article Alerts
Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Email Article

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Differences in the Antinociceptive Effects ofAlpha-2 Adrenoceptor Agonists in Two Substrains of Sprague-Dawley Rats
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
OtherANALGESIA AND DRUGS OF ABUSE

Differences in the Antinociceptive Effects ofAlpha-2 Adrenoceptor Agonists in Two Substrains of Sprague-Dawley Rats

Brent A. Graham, Donna L. Hammond and Herbert K. Proudfit
Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics November 1, 1997, 283 (2) 511-519;

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Share
OtherANALGESIA AND DRUGS OF ABUSE

Differences in the Antinociceptive Effects ofAlpha-2 Adrenoceptor Agonists in Two Substrains of Sprague-Dawley Rats

Brent A. Graham, Donna L. Hammond and Herbert K. Proudfit
Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics November 1, 1997, 283 (2) 511-519;
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Protein Kinase A Maintains Cellular Tolerance toMu Opioid Receptor Agonists in Hypothalamic Neurosecretory Cells with Chronic Morphine Treatment: Convergence on a Common Pathway with Estrogen in Modulating Mu Opioid Receptor/Effector Coupling
  • Structure-Activity Relationships of Indole- and Pyrrole-Derived Cannabinoids
  • Effect of COX-1 and COX-2 Inhibition on Induction and Maintenance of Carrageenan-Evoked Thermal Hyperalgesia in Rats
Show more ANALGESIA AND DRUGS OF ABUSE

Similar Articles

  • Home
  • Alerts
Facebook   Twitter   LinkedIn   RSS

Navigate

  • Current Issue
  • Fast Forward by date
  • Fast Forward by section
  • Latest Articles
  • Archive
  • Search for Articles
  • Feedback
  • ASPET

More Information

  • About JPET
  • Editorial Board
  • Instructions to Authors
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Customized Alerts
  • RSS Feeds
  • Subscriptions
  • Permissions
  • Terms & Conditions of Use

ASPET's Other Journals

  • Drug Metabolism and Disposition
  • Molecular Pharmacology
  • Pharmacological Reviews
  • Pharmacology Research & Perspectives
ISSN 1521-0103 (Online)

Copyright © 2021 by the American Society for Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics