INTRODUCTION

Amphetamine-like psychostimulants have multiple effects on dopamine (DA) neurons. By interacting with the DA transporter (DAT), these drugs are known to cause DA release and/or to block DA reuptake. Owing to the presence of negative feedback mechanisms, the increase in DA release also leads to an inhibition of firing of DA neurons (Bunney et al, 1973b; Einhorn et al, 1988; Shi et al, 2000a). This feedback inhibition alters following repeated psychostimulant administration and has, thus, been suggested to play a role in behaviors associated with psychostimulant abuse (Ellinwood and Lee, 1983; Kamata and Rebec, 1983; White and Wang, 1984; Henry et al, 1989; Wolf et al, 1993). We have recently shown that when the DA-mediated feedback inhibition is blocked by a DA antagonist such as raclopride, D-amphetamine, instead of producing no effect, powerfully excites DA cells (Shi et al, 2000b). Results further suggest that the effect is, in part, due to the activation of adrenergic α1 receptors.

Although the excitatory effect of D-amphetamine is normally masked by the dominant DA-mediated inhibition, in nonanesthetized rats chronically treated with D-amphetamine, half of DA neurons recorded in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) have been shown to be excited by an acute injection of D-amphetamine (Kamata and Rebec, 1984), suggesting that the excitatory effect of D-amphetamine may become dominant in some VTA DA neurons following repeated administration. A similar situation may occur during early development when negative feedback mechanisms of DA neurons are not fully developed (Trent et al, 1991) and in DAT-knockout mice in which DA-mediated feedback mechanisms are desensitized (Jones et al, 1999). The α1-mediated excitation may also provide part of the explanation as to why D-amphetamine remains effective in increasing DA release in DAT-knockout mice (Carboni et al, 2001), and why the drug is less effective in producing the same effect in α1b receptor-knockout mice (Auclair et al, 2002).

The present study was initially designed to examine whether other psychostimulants such as cocaine mimic the excitatory effect of D-amphetamine. In the course of data analysis, we noticed that the pattern of spike distribution induced by cocaine was not predicted by an increase in firing rate. Furthermore, the GABAA agonist muscimol, which also increased DA cell firing (Walters and Lakoski, 1978; Grace and Bunney, 1979; Waszczak and Walters, 1980) and DA release (Xi and Stein, 1998), produced a pattern very different from that induced by cocaine. Here, we provide evidence that cocaine not only increases firing rate and bursting but also induces a slow oscillation in DA neurons. Muscimol, on the other hand, suppresses the slow oscillation. Some results have been previously presented in abstract form (Shi et al, 2003).

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Single Unit Recordings In Vivo

All procedures were performed in accordance with protocols approved by the Yale Animal Care and Use Committee. Male Sprague–Dawley rats, weighing between 250 and 400 g, were anesthetized with chloral hydrate (400 mg/kg, i.p., with supplemental doses administered via a lateral tail vein). DA neurons in the VTA were identified and recorded extracellularly using techniques described previously (Bunney et al, 1973b; Grace and Bunney, 1983; Shi et al, 2000b). Interspike intervals (ISIs) were collected on line via an interface (Lab-PC+, National Instrument, Austin, Taxes) to a PC computer using software written in LabView for Windows. In a subset of animals, ipsilateral forebrain hemisections (4 mm posterior to bregma) were made 30 min before beginning of recording (Shi et al, 2000a). Only one cell was studied in each rat.

Drugs

Drugs used in this study were cocaine hydrochloride, D-amphetamine sulfate, (+)-methamphetamine hydrochloride, methylphenidate hydrochloride, L-dopa methyl ester hydrochloride, nisoxetine hydrochloride, s(−)-raclopride (+)-tartrate, and prazosin hydrochloride. All were purchased from Sigma-RBI (St Louis, MO) and administered intravenously through a lateral tail vein. All doses given refer to the salts. Prazosin was dissolved in 25–30% poly(ethylene glycol) (PEG, average MW 200) at 2.5 mg/ml. Before injection, the solution was diluted with distilled water so that the final volume of injection was either 0.05 or 0.1 ml. All other drugs were dissolved in distilled water.

Data Analysis

All analyses were performed using Visual Basic macros in Microsoft Excel. The onset of a burst was identified as the concurrence of two spikes with an ISI less than 80 ms and the termination of a burst was defined as an ISI greater than 160 ms (Grace and Bunney, 1984). Two approaches were taken to measure firing periodicity of a cell. First, rate histograms were constructed with a bin width of 50 ms. Segments (102.4 s) of the rate histogram were then subjected to the fast Fourier transform (FFT), yielding spectra with a frequency resolution of 0.01 Hz. In the second approach, autocorrelograms were constructed with a bin width of 45 ms over 2048 bins using autocovariance computed from the same ISI data sets. Following tapering (2.5% data points on each end using a split-cosine-bell function) and removal of the linear trend, the FFT was performed to yield spectra with a resolution of 0.0109 Hz. Results of the two approaches were qualitatively identical. For simplicity, all spectral data reported in this study, unless noted, were from autocorrelograms.

To help visualize slow changes in rate histograms and autocorrelograms (eg, Figure 2a and b), data were smoothed using the following function:

where yi is the smoothed value, xi is the original data, xi−1 and xi+1 are values immediately before and after xi, respectively, and α is the smoothing factor (0<α<1). For the purpose of this study, each data series was repeatedly smoothed by a factor of 0.4 for 10 times. The number of repeat serves a role similar to that of window size in a widow smoothing function. Unlike commonly used window functions, however, this method does not introduce rhythmic background noises to the final spectrum. As might be expected, spectra from smoothed data were nearly identical to those from unsmoothed results in the low-frequency range. However, peaks at high frequencies (>3 Hz) were significantly suppressed. Since we were also interested in oscillations higher than 3 Hz, all spectral results reported in this study were from unsmoothed data.

Figure 2
figure 2

Slow oscillation and effects of cocaine and muscimol. (a1) Portions of the rate histogram of the cell shown in Figure 1a reconstructed with a bin width of 50 ms (top: after raclopride; bottom: after cocaine). To help visualizing slow changes, data were smoothed and results were shown in thick lines. Note the oscillation in firing after cocaine injection (about seven times per 10 s, that is, 0.7 Hz). Y-axis unit: spikes/s. (a2) Spectra computed from the rate histograms. Consistent with the observation in (a1), a major peak was detected at 0.74 Hz after cocaine injection. Y-axis unit: (spikes/s)2/Hz. (b1) Portions of autocorrelograms (autocovariance) with the linear trend removed and data at the beginning tapered (see Material and methods). Thin and thick lines are the raw and smoothed data, respectively. Y-axis unit: (spikes/s)2. (b2) Power spectra of the autocorrelograms showing again a major peak at 0.74 Hz after cocaine injection. Y-axis unit: (spikes/s)4/Hz. (c1) Autocorrelograms from the cell shown in Figure 1b. (c2) Power spectra of the autocorrelograms showing a decrease in the low-frequency oscillation (0.67 Hz, double arrow) and an increase in the high-frequency (6.43 Hz, single arrow) oscillation by muscimol (Mus). The insets are the same spectra displayed on an expanded Y-axis scale to show the presence of the slow oscillation (0.67 Hz) under baseline conditions (top inset, double arrow) and its suppression by muscimol (bottom inset).

The statistical significance of an effect of a drug was determined by comparing measures before and after drug injection using ANCOVA followed by a post hoc Tukey test. The covariate was the baseline value obtained prior to the first drug injection. Spectral data were log-transformed before subjected to statistical comparison. All numerical data were expressed as the mean±SEM.

RESULTS

Effects of Cocaine on Spike Distribution of DA Neurons

D-Amphetamine has been shown to excite DA neurons when the DA-mediated feedback inhibition is blocked (Shi et al, 2000b). To determine whether cocaine mimics the effect, VTA DA neurons were recorded in rats pretreated with the D2/3 antagonist raclopride (0.1–0.2 mg/kg). In all cells tested, cocaine (1 mg/kg) increased firing rate (from 4.7±0.4 to 6.0±0.4 spikes/s, n=15, p<0.001, Figure 1a).

Figure 1
figure 1

Different firing patterns induced by cocaine and muscimol. (a) Top: Typical rate histogram showing excitation of a DA neuron induced by cocaine (Coc) given after raclopride (Rac). Bin width: 10 s. Y-axis unit: spikes/10 s. Bottom: plots of instantaneous firing rate (1/ISIs) showing an increase in ISI variation during cocaine-induced excitation. Each dot represents one ISI. (b) Recordings from a different DA cell showing that the GABAA agonist muscimol (Mus) also increased firing rate (top), but it decreased ISI variation (bottom). (c) In another DA cell, muscimol produced similar effects when given after raclopride.

To monitor continuously changes in firing pattern, instantaneous firing rates (1/ISI) were plotted against time (Figure 1a). Unexpectedly, the increase in firing rate induced by cocaine was associated with an increase in not only short but also long ISIs. Consequently, the variation coefficient of ISIs was significantly increased (from 49.2±4.8 to 76.4±7.1% of the mean ISI, n=15, p<0.001). To determine whether the change is common when a DA neuron is excited, the GABAA agonist muscimol was administered. Confirming previous results (Walters and Lakoski, 1978; Grace and Bunney, 1979; Waszczak and Walters, 1980), muscimol (2 mg/kg) increased firing rate in all DA cells tested (from 4.4±0.6 to 5.8±0.6 spikes/s, n=8, p<0.001). Unlike cocaine, however, muscimol decreased the variation coefficient of ISIs from 54.4±3.0 to 25.3±5.3% (p<0.001, Figure 1b). Similar results were obtained when muscimol was given after raclopride (0.2 mg/kg, n=8, Figure 1c), suggesting that raclopride used in the cocaine experiments is not directly responsible for different results obtained with cocaine and muscimol.

Effects of Cocaine and Muscimol on Oscillatory Properties of DA Neurons

An increase in ISI variation indicates an increased irregularity of firing. However, the general pattern of spike distribution induced by cocaine seemed to repeat over time (Figure 1a), suggesting that the large variation in ISIs induced by cocaine may not be entirely random and may contain periodic components. To test this possibility, FFT analysis was performed on rate histograms constructed with a small bin width (50 ms, Figure 2a) and on autocorrelograms using autocovariances (unstandardized autocorrelation) computed from the same ISI data sets (Figure 2b). The two approaches yielded qualitatively identical results; both showed a large increase by cocaine in the mean power between 0.5 and 1.5 Hz (PLow, Figure 2a and b). The mean power between 3 and 10 Hz (PHigh, Figure 3), however, was unaffected, suggesting that cocaine selectively increases the slow oscillation of DA neurons. Muscimol, on the other hand, suppressed the slow (PLow) and increased the fast oscillations (PHigh, Figures 2c and 3).

Figure 3
figure 3

Summary bar graphs showing effects of cocaine and muscimol on firing properties of DA neurons. Four different parameters were measured: firing rate (spikes/10 s), level of bursting (spikes in bursts/10 s), PLow (mean power between 0.5 and 1.5 Hz), and PHigh (mean power between 3 and 10 Hz). Values are expressed as percent changes of baseline. Open and filled bars represent mean values before and after cocaine or muscimol, respectively. Left: cocaine (n=15), given after raclopride, significantly increased firing rate, spikes in bursts, and PLow, and had no effect on PHigh. Center: muscimol (n=8) increased firing rate and PHigh, and decreased PLow. Similar effects were observed when muscimol was given after raclopride (not shown). Right: in prazosin-pretreated rats (right, n=6), cocaine, given after raclopride, produced no significant effects on all four measures. Note that Y-axis are in log scale. ****p<0.001 compared to baseline (center chart) or values obtained after raclopride (left and right charts).

As shown in Figure 2c, some DA cells exhibited a clear slow oscillation even under baseline conditions. To determine the significance of the activity, we compared PLow with the mean power of the adjacent frequencies (0–3 Hz, P0–3 Hz). In cells tested with cocaine, PLow was significantly higher than P0–3 Hz in one out 15 cells under baseline conditions. After raclopride, the number of cells showing significantly higher PLow was increased to 3. Following cocaine, the number was further increased to 9.

Adrenergic α1 receptors have been shown to play a critical role in the excitatory effect of D-amphetamine on DA neurons (Shi et al, 2000b). To test whether they are also involved in the effect of cocaine, rats were pretreated with the α1 antagonist prazosin (0.5 mg/kg, n=6). While having no effect by itself, prazosin prevented cocaine, given after raclopride, from increasing both firing rate and the slow oscillation (PLow, Figure 3).

Relations between the Slow Oscillation and Bursting

Like D-amphetamine (Shi et al, 2000b), cocaine increased bursting of DA neurons (Figure 3). This raises the possibility that the observed slow oscillation is an epiphenomenon of repetitive bursting. Inconsistent with this possibility, the frequency of bursting and the frequency of the slow oscillation were uncorrelated (Figure 4a). There was, however, a significant and positive correlation between the frequency of bursting and the amplitude of the slow oscillation (PLow, Figure 4b). Thus, bursting itself might be a phenomenon secondary to the slow oscillation, or the two measures reflect different aspects of the same process and/or share some common mechanisms.

Figure 4
figure 4

Relations between bursting and slow oscillations. There was no significant correlation between the frequency of bursting and the frequency of the slow oscillation (top). A significant correlation was observed, however, between the frequency of bursting and PLow (bottom).

Effects of D-Amphetamine, Methamphetamine, and Methylphenidate

To test whether the effect of cocaine is shared by other psychostimulants, D-amphetamine, methamphetamine, and methylphenidate were administered. All three psychostimulants inhibited DA neurons when given before raclopride (data not shown). When given after raclopride (0.1–0.2 mg/kg), all three psychostimulants (D-amphetamine, 1 mg/kg, n=16; methamphetamine, 1 mg/kg, n=6; and methylphenidate, 1–2.5 mg/kg, n=15) significantly increased PLow (Figure 5). Furthermore, the increase was reversed by prazosin in all cells tested (Figure 5a and b).

Figure 5
figure 5

Effects of D-amphetamine, methamphetamine, and methylphenidate on DA neurons. (a) Plots of instantaneous firing rates showing that D-amphetamine (Amp), given after raclopride (Rac), mimicked the effect of cocaine and increased the variation of ISIs. Prazosin (Praz) reversed the effect (see also spectra in b). (b) Power spectra of autocorrelograms showing an increase in slow oscillations induced by both raclopride (insets, double arrow) and D-amphetamine, and the reversal of the increase by prazosin. Note that the effect of raclopride was much smaller than that of D-amphetamine (insets), and the effect of prazosin is more clearly shown here than in (a). Y-axis unit: (spikes/s)4/Hz. (c) Summary bar graphs showing increases in PLow by D-amphetamine (left, n=16), methamphetamine (center, n=6), and methylphenidate (right, n=15). Values are expressed as percent changes of baseline. Open and filled bars represent mean values before and after the injection of one of the psychostimulants, respectively. All three psychostimulants also increased firing rate and bursting. The effect of methylphenidate on firing rate, however, was statistically insignificant. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.005, ****p<0.001 compared to values obtained after raclopride.

Effects of Nisoxetine

Results with prazosin suggest that norepinephrine (NE) is a key mediator of the effect of psychostimulants. To test whether an increase in NE release alone can lead to an increase in the slow oscillation, the selective NE uptake blocker nisoxetine was administered before (n=5) or after raclopride (0.1 mg/kg, n=10). In both groups, nisoxetine (10 mg/kg) increased PLow (top charts in Figure 6). The effect was smaller, however, compared to the effect of psychostimulants. The increase induced by psychostimulants was approximately 44% (methylphenidate) to 738% (cocaine) higher than that induced by nisoxetine (Figures 3 and 5).

Figure 6
figure 6

Effects of nisoxetine, L-dopa, and forebrain hemisections on DA neurons. The NE uptake blocker nisoxetine increased bursting and PLow when given before (top left, n=5) or after raclopride (top right, n=10). L-Dopa produced no effect on all four measures when given after raclopride (bottom left, n=13). Following forebrain hemisections, D-amphetamine, given after raclopride, also had no effect on bursting and PLow. It, however, increased firing rate (bottom right, n=8). Values are expressed as percent changes of baseline. Open and filled bars represent, respectively, mean values before and after the injection of nisoxetine, L-dopa, or D-amphetamine. *p<0.05, ***p<0.005, ****p<0.001 compared to baseline (upper left chart) or values after raclopride (upper right and lower charts).

Effects of L-Dopa

Previous studies suggest that DA release in the VTA activates not only DA autoreceptors but also adrenergic α1 receptors (Grenhoff et al, 1995; Paladini et al, 2001) and a DAT-mediated inward current in DA neurons (Ingram et al, 2002). When DA autoreceptors are blocked, DA has been shown to excite DA neurons through the two non-DA receptor mechanisms. To test whether the two mechanisms are also responsible for the slow oscillation induced by psychostimulants, L-dopa was administered. L-Dopa, by increasing DA release, should activate both mechanisms. As observed previously (Bunney et al, 1973a), L-dopa (100 mg/kg) inhibited DA cell firing when given alone (from 4.8±0.5 to 2.8±0.3 spikes/s, n=8, p<0.001), and the inhibition was completely reversed by raclopride, confirming that L-dopa can cause DA release and activate DA autoreceptors. When given after raclopride (0.1–0.2 mg/kg), however, L-dopa produced (100 mg/kg) no significant effect on either firing rate or PLow (n=13, lower left chart in Figure 6). Thus, VTA DA release and activation of the two non-DA receptor mechanisms may be necessary, but not enough for a drug to cause the slow oscillation in DA neurons.

Effects of Forebrain Hemisection

To test further whether the generation of the slow oscillation requires activation of more than just local mechanisms in the VTA, forebrain hemisections were made to interrupt forebrain inputs to DA neurons. In all cells tested, D-amphetamine (1 mg/kg), given after raclopride (0.1–0.2 mg/kg), failed to produce a significant effect on the slow oscillation (PLow), although in the same cells, it increased firing rate (n=8, lower right chart in Figure 6).

DISCUSSION

We have previously shown that D-amphetamine has two opposing effects on the firing activity of DA neurons: a DA-mediated inhibition and a non-DA-mediated excitation (Shi et al, 2000b). Results of the present study show that the two effects are shared by all psychostimulants tested. Using spectral analysis, this study further shows that during psychostimulant-induced excitation, DA cells exhibit not only an increase in firing rate and bursting but also a slow rhythmic oscillation. The increase in firing is unlikely to be the direct cause of the slow oscillation since the GABAA agonist muscimol, which also increases DA cell firing, suppresses the slow oscillation. The nonpsychostimulant DA agonist L-dopa also fails to mimic the effect of psychostimulants. Further evidence suggests that the slow oscillation induced by psychostimulants requires the activation of α1 receptors and depends on intact forebrain inputs to DA neurons.

Possible Mechanisms Underlying the Slow Oscillation Induced by Psychostimulants

The blockade of the slow oscillation by the α1 antagonist prazosin and its mimic by the selective NE transporter blocker nisoxetine suggest that the effect is mediated, at least in part, by NE. The effect of nisoxetine, however, was less pronounced compared to that of psychostimulants. While this may be related to pharmacokinetic properties of different drugs, psychostimulants, by increasing release of both NE and DA, may induce more extensive α1 activation than nisoxetine. Previous studies suggest that DA may act as an NE agonist (Cornil et al, 2002). Unexpectedly, L-dopa, a precursor for both DA and NE, failed to mimic the effect of psychostimulants. One possibility is that L-dopa preferentially increases DA release. DA is synthesized in the cytoplasm and can be released via the DAT-mediated reverse transport when cytoplasmic DA concentration is high. NE, on the other hand, is synthesized within synaptic vesicles. Its release may therefore be less influenced by the cytoplasmic concentration of DA and more dependent on the firing rate of the cell. Studies have shown that L-dopa has no effect (Aoki et al, 1987) or decreases NE release (Dayan and Finberg, 2003).

The failure of L-dopa to mimic the effect of psychostimulants also supports the suggestion that DA release in the VTA is not enough to cause the slow oscillation. Previous studies suggest that VTA DA release activates not only DA autoreceptors but also α1 receptors (Grenhoff et al, 1995; Paladini et al, 2001) and the DAT-mediated inward current (Ingram et al, 2002). Thus, in the presence of DA receptor blockade, psychostimulants may act through the two non-DA receptor mechanisms to induce the slow oscillation. If this suggestion is correct, L-dopa, by increasing DA release, should mimic the effect. In this study, L-dopa was shown to mimic only the DA-mediated inhibition and not the slow oscillation, suggesting that DA release and activation of the two non-DA receptor mechanisms may be necessary, but not enough for a drug to induce the slow oscillation. The observation that disruption of forebrain inputs to DA neurons prevented D-amphetamine from inducing the slow oscillation, but not the DA-mediated inhibition (Shi et al, 2000a) further supports the suggestion that psychostimulant-induced slow oscillation involves activation of more than just local mechanisms.

Results with forebrain hemisections also suggest that forebrain inputs may play a critical role in the slow oscillation induced by psychostimulants. Several forebrain areas are known to project directly to VTA DA neurons, including the prefrontal cortex (PFC), hippocampus, amygdala, nucleus accumbens, and ventral pallidum. The input from the PFC is of particularly interesting. Darracq et al (1998) have shown that part of DA release induced by D-amphetamine is DAT independent and can be blocked by prazosin locally injected into the PFC, suggesting that NE release and α1 receptor activation in the PFC may excite DA neurons, causing the DAT-independent DA release. Some PFC neurons also display a slow oscillatory activity at frequencies similar to that of the slow oscillation observed in VTA (Cowan et al, 1994; Lewis and O'Donnell, 2000). In preliminary experiments, transections immediately posterior to the PFC reduced the effect of D-amphetamine on the slow oscillations. Taken together, these results suggest that the PFC might be an important component in the neuronal circuitry that mediates the slow oscillation in DA neurons.

Potential Significance of the Slow Oscillation

The reinforcing effects of psychostimulants have been largely attributed to their ability to increase DA release. Both L-dopa and muscimol also increase DA release. Neither, however, has significant reinforcing efficacies (Patkina and Lapin, 1976; Poncelet et al, 1983; Xi and Stein, 1998), suggesting that not all drugs capable of increasing DA release are reinforcing. The slow conversion of L-dopa to DA and the GABAA action of muscimol may explain the lack of reinforcing efficacies of the two drugs. Results of this study raise another possibility. The reinforcing properties of psychostimulants may depend on both DA release and activation of the network underlying the slow oscillation. Thus, the inability of L-dopa and muscimol to produce the latter effect may contribute to the lack of reinforcing properties of the two drugs.

Oscillatory firing could affect both DA release from DA terminals and information processing in DA soma and dendrites. Gonon (1988) showed that a rhythmic, low-frequency (1 Hz) burst stimulation is up to six times more potent than evenly spaced stimulation in evoking DA release. Oscillations in firing may also lead to an oscillatory release. Grace (2000) suggests that phasic and tonic DA release may serve different functions. Oscillatory release also provides a means for DA terminals to differentially interact other inputs to the same target neurons. Inputs that coincide with the oscillatory activity of DA cells may be affected more by DA than those that do not. At the cell body area, oscillations may enable a neuron to act as a coincidence detector and to become more sensitive to the precise timing of synaptic inputs (Volgushev et al, 1998). In cells with oscillations, discharges evoked by synaptic inputs has been shown to be phase locked to the oscillation cycle (Volgushev et al, 1998). These properties, together with the observation that precise synchronization is often associated with oscillatory responses, suggest that oscillations are instrumental for synchrony between spatially distributed neurons.

It should be pointed out that the slow oscillation induced by psychostimulants was observed when DA-mediated feedback inhibition was blocked by raclopride. In rats without raclopride, psychostimulants inhibited DA cells. Accompanied with this inhibition, PLow was either unchanged or decreased (data not shown), suggesting that the DA-mediated inhibition prevents the expression of the slow oscillation. However, when a cell stops firing or fires at a low rate, the slow oscillation may occur mainly at subthreshold levels, expressed as changes in the membrane potential and not fully reflected in firing rate. Owing to this, it may not be detected by a technique relying on analysis of spikes. Thus, a direct measurement of the membrane potential will be required to provide more convincing evidence for whether or not the activity is present when D2-like receptors are not blocked.

In adult rats anesthetized with chloral hydrate, the DA-mediated inhibition induced by a psychostimulant is more dominant than the excitatory effect induced by the same drug. However, as cited in the introduction, the inhibitory effect may weaken and the excitatory effect may become more dominant under certain conditions (Kamata and Rebec, 1984; Trent et al, 1991; Jones et al, 1999). It would be interesting to determine whether under these conditions, a psychostimulant can induce the slow oscillation without prior D2 blockade.

Regardless of whether the slow oscillation is expressed, activation of the network responsible for the activity and corresponding receptors on DA neurons may have delayed and possibly long-lasting effects. A previous study shows that a single injection of cocaine induces long-term potentiation of glutamate synapses on DA neurons (Ungless et al, 2001). It would be interesting to determine whether these synapses are part of the network mediating the slow oscillation, and whether the reported long-term potentiation is due to repeated, low-frequency activation of these synapses by cocaine.