INTRODUCTION

Prepulse inhibition (PPI) of the acoustic startle response can be measured in human subjects as well as in laboratory animals (Geyer et al, 2001; Swerdlow et al, 2001a). It has recently gained considerable attention as an animal model of gating deficits in schizophrenia (Geyer et al, 1990; Braff et al, 1992,2001). In this paradigm, brief presentation of a nonstartling prepulse stimulus, shortly prior to presentation of a startling stimulus, results in an inhibition of the startle response. As mentioned above, the inhibition of such response is disrupted in schizophrenic patients, and it can be altered in laboratory rats by manipulations that change brain neurotransmission. Among drugs that evoke strong impairments of PPI are substances affecting glutamatergic, dopaminergic and 5-HT (serotonergic) neurotransmission (for a review see Koch, 1999; Geyer et al, 2001). In addition to pharmacological manipulations, environmental factors have also been shown to disrupt PPI (see Geyer et al, 2001 for a review). It was evidenced that in normal adult human subjects stress was capable of diminishing PPI (Grillon and Davis, 1997), while corticotropin releasing factor (CRF), a neurotransmitter that is released during stress, diminished PPI in rats (Conti et al, 2002). Moreover, rearing rats in isolation, maternal separation and chronic stress have also been shown to attenuate PPI, although negative results have been reported as well (Geyer et al, 1993; Ellenbroek et al, 1998; Faraday et al, 1999; Sipos et al, 2000; Geyer et al, 2001, Weiss and Feldon, 2001). Nevertheless, the results cited above raised the question about the role of stress and stress hormones in the exacerbation of symptoms of schizophrenia (Walker and Diforio, 1997; Walder et al, 2000).

Corticosterone is the main glucocorticosteroid hormone produced by the rat adrenal glands in response to stress. The released corticosterone readily enters the central nervous system (McEwen et al, 1968), where it binds to two types of intracellular receptors (de Kloet et al, 1990): high affinity mineralocorticoid receptors (MR) and lower affinity glucocorticoid receptors (GR). In the rat brain, MR receptors are present mainly in the hippocampus, in contrast to GR receptors, which have much more widespread distribution (Harfstrand et al, 1986; de Kloet et al, 1990; Ahima et al, 1992), being localized in many areas important for PPI control (Sipes and Geyer, 1995; Fendt et al, 2001; Fletcher et al, 2001; Swerdlow et al, 2001a,2001b), such as the hippocampus, cerebral cortex, amygdala, mesolimbic areas, nuclei of monoaminergic neurons and brain stem nuclei. This suggests that corticosterone may have direct effects on PPI or can act via modulation of brain neurotransmission.

Changes in glucocorticoid level were shown to influence potently the 5-HT neurotransmission, especially at the level of 5-HT1A receptors in the hippocampus. As evidenced by electrophysiological studies, both adrenalectomy (ADX) and corticosterone treatment modulated responses to 5-HT1A receptor stimulation in the hippocampal neurons (Joels and de Kloet, 1992; Karten et al, 1999; Mueller and Beck, 2000; Czyrak et al, 2002). The ADX animals showed rapid increase in the synthesis of 5-HT1A receptor mRNA, and an increase in 5-HT1A receptor binding in the hippocampus, and both these effects were prevented by low concentrations of corticosterone (Mendelson and McEwen, 1992a; Chalmers et al, 1994; Kuroda et al, 1994; Tejani-Butt and Labow, 1994). All these studies show that the 5-HT1A receptors (at least in the hippocampus) are under glucocorticoids control. They also suggest that in psychiatric pathologies with characteristic elevation of adrenocorticoid levels, the ability of these hormones to change 5-HT1A receptors may be included among the factors that modulate clinical symptoms.

Although schizophrenia and coexisting cognitive deficits are traditionally linked with dysfunction of dopamine and 5-HT2A receptors (for review see Harrison, 1999), recent clinical data suggest also the involvement of 5-HT1A receptors (Burnet et al, 1996; Bantick et al, 2001). Moreover, experimental data show that direct agonists of 5-HT1A receptors are able to disrupt PPI (Rigdon and Weatherspoon, 1992; Sipes and Geyer, 1994,1995; Fletcher et al, 2001; Prinssen et al, 2002). The role of 5-HT1A receptors in modulation of sensorimotor gating mechanisms is further supported by the fact that disturbances in PPI induced by psychotomimetic substances (such as MK-801) are attenuated by 5-HT1A receptor antagonists (Wedzony et al, 2000).

Above arguments prompted us to investigate whether the treatment with corticosterone, the main glucocorticoid hormone in rats, would be able to influence process of sensorimotor gating measured as PPI. Further, our study evaluated the effect of corticosterone on 8-OHDPAT-evoked disruption of PPI, on the assumption that the results of such experiment would reflect the effect of corticosterone on 5-HT1A receptor-mediated cognitive dysfunction expressed as disruption of sensorimotor gating. In the light of the hypothesis of Sipes and Geyer (1995) that the disruptive effects of 8-OHDPAT on PPI were (at least in part) due to its ability to decrease serotonergic neuronal activity, the impact of corticosterone on the level of 5-HT and its metabolite 5-HIAA in vehicle and 8-OHDPAT-treated rats was also assessed. Finally we examined whether the applied regimen of corticosterone treatment has an impact on 5-HT1A receptors in the brain regions, which have been shown to be implicated in the regulation of PPI. For this purpose, 5-HT1A receptor mRNA and the binding of [3H]8-OHDPAT to those receptors were analyzed in the dorsal and ventral part of hippocampus, entorhinal cortex and raphe nuclei.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals and Drug Treatments

Male Wistar rats (250–300 g) were used in all the experiments. The animals were housed in groups of six under controlled conditions (12 h light/dark cycle, 22±2°C). The rats were allowed at least one week habituation to the animal room, and had free access to a standard laboratory diet and tap water. Corticosterone-21-acetate (Sigma, St Louis, MO) was prepared as a suspension in 1% Tween 80 and was administered at a dose of 10 mg/kg s.c., acutely (single dose), or repeatedly (twice a day for 7 days). All the experiments were performed (or rats were decapitated) 2 h (acute treatment), or 2 and 24 h (repeated treatment) after the last injection of corticosterone or vehicle. The experimental protocols were approved by the Committee for Laboratory Animal Welfare and Ethics of the Institute of Pharmacology, Polish Academy of Sciences in Kraków and met the criteria of the International Council for Laboratory Animals, Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.

Measurement of Serum Corticosterone

The animals were decapitated and their blood samples were collected from the trunk to determine serum corticosterone levels. Corticosterone levels were measured by a radioimmunoassay (Przegalinski et al, 1992), using rabbit antiserum raised against corticosterone-21-thyroglobulin (Sigma, St Louis, MO) as an antigen. The antiserum showed a very low cross-reactivity with other major steroids. The assay sensitivity was 10 pg/tube.

FUNCTIONAL STUDIES

PPI of Acoustic Startle Response

The apparatus for measuring of the startle response was placed in a sound-proof cabinet, and consisted of three identical (17 × 30 × 17 cm), transparent Plexiglas cages (Columbus Instruments, OH), which were equipped with a moveable platform floor attached to a sensor recording vertical movements of the floor (Wedzony et al, 1994,2000). A startle reflex was evoked by acoustic stimuli delivered from a loudspeaker suspended above the cage and connected to an acoustic generator. The transient force resulting from movements of the platform, evoked by the startle reaction of rat to acoustic stimuli, was recorded with a PC computer during a recording window of 200 ms, measured from the onset of the acoustic stimuli, and was then digitalized and stored in the computer for further evaluation. The amplitude of the startle response was defined as a difference between the maximum force detected during a recording window and the force measured immediately before the stimulus onset (expressed in arbitrary units). The threshold level was set at 10 g, so that responses exceeding a force of 10 g would activate the automatic peak detection software. The threshold of 10 g allowed a correct evaluation of the maximum response in all the tested animals. The amplitudes were averaged for each individual animal, separately for both types of trials (stimulus alone or stimulus preceded by prepulse).

Prior to drug administration, the animals were exposed to a single startle session (identical to that described below). The purpose of that procedure was to familiarize the animals with the test cages and, consequently, to produce a more stable baseline of startle/PPI responding and secondly, to make sure that the animals were allocated to the balanced groups in terms of equivalent startle amplitude and PPI. On a test day, the rats were injected with the vehicle or drug, and after an appropriate time they were placed into the testing cages. The experimental session consisted of a 5-min habituation to the 65 dB background white noise (continuous throughout the session), followed by a test session. During the test session, each rat was confronted with two types of trials: (1) 120 dB, 40 ms pulse, (2) 75 dB, 20 ms prepulse followed 80 ms later by a 120 dB, 40 ms pulse (all pulses had 4000 Hz frequency). Each trial type was presented 20 times during the session with 20 s intertrial intervals. Thus, the session consisted of two blocks of 20 trials, with trial order arranged according to a Latin square design. The animals' responses to each trial type in each block were averaged out for every single animal and used for further calculations. The degree of PPI was shown as a percentage score (%PPI) calculated as ([(P)−(PP+P)/P] × 100) a difference between the amplitude of the pulse alone (P) and the amplitude of the prepulse+pulse trials (PP+P), divided by the amplitude of the pulse alone trials and multiplied by 100 (Wedzony et al, 1994). A high value of the calculated %PPI indicated that the prepulse inhibited the response to a pulse stimulus, whereas a low value indicated weaker inhibition by prepulse.

8-OHDPAT (0.2, 1.25, 5.0 mg/kg s.c.) was given 15 min before beginning of the test session. WAY 100135 (2.5, 5.0, 20.0 mg/kg s.c.) was given 30 min before 8-OHDPAT; the last dose of corticosterone was administered 2 and 24 h before 8-OHDPAT. Independent groups of animals were used for all the drug treatments.

Measurement of 5-HT and 5-HIAA Concentrations

The rats treated chronically with corticosterone or respective vehicle were killed by decapitation 24 h after the last dose. One hour before decapitation, respective groups received 8-OHDPAT (1.25 mg/kg s.c.) or saline injection. The brains were quickly removed and parts of the brains containing raphe nuclei were cut into 1-mm coronal slices using a Rodent Brain Matrix (Activational Systems Inc., Michigan) from which raphe nuclei were dissected with a punching tube (0.5 mm in diameter) as described previously (Wedzony et al, 1997). The hippocampus was isolated from the rest of the brain. The tissue samples were stored at −70oC until assay. On the day of assay, the tissue samples were homogenized in 0.1 M HClO4 containing 0.26 mM ascorbic acid, centrifuged (14 000 g), and filtered through 0.2 μm cellulose membranes. To determine the 5-HT and 5-HIAA levels, the 10 μl sample aliquots were injected onto a HPLC column (ODS, Phase II, particle size 3 μm, 3.2/100 mm; BAS). A standard HPLC technique (BAS 400, Bioanalytical Systems, USA) with an electrochemical detection (BAS- LC-4B detector with an oxidation potential of 0.65 V against Ag/AgCl reference electrode and a glassy-carbon working electrode TL5) was used. The mobile phase (flow rate 0.8 ml/min) consisted of 0.06 M citrate-phosphate buffer (pH 3.5), 0.27 mM EDTA, 0.32 mM sodium octyl sulfonate and 2.4% methanol. The 5-HT and 5-HIAA levels were quantified by a peak area comparison with standards, run on the day of the analysis, using Inject software. A histological verification of brain areas was carried out in slices transferred into a phosphate buffer containing 4% paraformaldehyde.

RECEPTOR STUDIES

Preparation of Brain Sections

The rats were decapitated 2 or 24 h after the last vehicle or corticosterone injection, the brains were removed and rapidly cut into coronal blocks, containing the hippocampus or raphe nuclei with entorhinal cortex, frozen by submersion in dry-ice cold isopentane and stored at −70°C until sectioning. Coronal sections (10 μm) were cut on a cryostat (Leica LC 3000), thaw-mounted onto subbed glass microscope slides and stored at −20°C.

5HT1A Receptor Autoradiography

At the beginning, the sections were preincubated (30 min, room temperature) in 50 mM Tris-HCl buffer (pH 7.4) containing (in mM) 180 NaCl, 5 KCl, 2.5 CaCl2, and 1.2 MgCl2. Binding assay was carried out (Wedzony et al, 1997) by incubating the sections for 60 min (room temperature) with 1.5 nM [3H]8-OHDPAT (Amersham, specific activity137 mCi/mmol) in the same 50 mM Tris-HCl buffer supplemented with ascorbic acid (10 mg/1 dl) and pargyline (0.2 mg/1 dl). Nonspecific binding was determined in adjacent sections under the same conditions except for the presence of 5 μM 5-HT. After incubation the sections were rinsed, dried, and apposed to a Hyperfilm-3H (Amersham) along with a tritium standard (3H-microscale, Amersham) in an X-ray cassette for 3 weeks at 4°C. The films were then developed with D-19 Kodak developer. Autoradiographs were quantified by a computerized densitometry (Image Analyzing System, Image ProPlus, Media Cybernetics, USA). The measured light transmittance values were converted into a tissue equivalent radioactivity (with reference to standards), and thus to binding values (fmol/mg tissue, with reference to the specific activity) of the radiolabeled ligand. Values of the nonspecific binding were determined directly from film images of the sections incubated in the presence of the displacer 5-HT (5 μM). Those values were averaged and subtracted from the total binding to obtain specific binding values. Anatomical regions were verified according to the Paxinos and Watson atlas (1998) in tissue sections stained with cresyl violet.

In Situ Hybridization Histochemistry

Prior to hybridization, the tissue sections were fixed in 0.01 M phosphate-buffered saline (pH 7.0) containing 4% paraformaldehyde (10 min, 0°C). The slides were then washed and acetylated with a mixture of 0.1 M triethanolamine (pH 8) and 0.25% acetic anhydride for 10 min and subsequently dehydrated in graded ethanol, defatted in chloroform, rinsed in graded ethanol and air-dried. For the in situ hybridization assay, a mixture of two 30-mer deoxyoligonucleotide probes (Genset., USA), corresponding to amino-acid sequences 175–184 and 262–271 of the rat 5-HT1A receptor, was used (Liao et al, 1993). A homology search (Fast GENBANK) showed no significant homology with any known protein. The probes were labeled with [α-35S]dATP (NEN, Belgium) using a terminal transferase (Boehringer-Mannheim, Germany), purified chromatographically (Micro Bio-Spin Chromatography columns, BioRad, USA), and stored (3 × 105 dpm/μl) at −20°C until further use. The sections were hybridized with the [35S]dATP-labeled probe (100 μl/slide), diluted to a final activity of about 2.45 × 105 dpm/ml in a hybridization buffer (50% deionized formamide, 4 × SSC, 25 mM sodium phosphate, 1 mM sodium pyrophosphate, 1 × Denhardt's solution, 200 μg/ml salmon sperm DNA, 100 μg/ml polyadenylic acid, 10% dextran sulfate, 250 μg/ml yeast tRNA) for 20 h at 42°C in a humidified incubator (Wedzony et al, 2000). After incubation, the sections were rinsed in 1 × SSC (room temperature), washed 3 times in 1 × SSC at 55°C for 15 min, then in 1 × SSC at room temperature for 30 min, and rinsed finally in 0.1 × SSC and in sterile water. After washes, the slides were dehydrated in graded ethanol, dried, and exposed to an X-ray film (Hyperfilm-βmax Amersham) for 20 days at 4°C and processed with a D-19 Kodak developer. The specificity of the hybridization signal was confirmed by visualizing silver grains over individual cell bodies. To this end, the slides were immersed in a photographic emulsion (Kodak NBT 3 emulsion) and left in darkness for 12 weeks. After processing with D-19 Kodak developer they were counterstained with cresyl violet. The autoradiographs were analyzed using an image analysing system (SPOT-32 camera and Image ProPlus). For quantification, the mean optical density of a specific hybridization signal was measured in the regions of interest.

DRUGS, DATA PRESENTATION, AND STATISTICS

The following drugs and substances were used: corticosterone 21-acetate (Sigma, St Louis), (±)8-OHDPAT hydrobromide (RBI, Natic, MA), and (S)WAY 100135 (Tocris, UK). All the other reagents and corticosterone antiserum were purchased from Sigma (St Louis, MO).

The data were presented as the mean±SEM and were evaluated statistically using STATISTICA software. One-way ANOVA was used to analyze: (1) behavioral experiments showing dose–response of 8-OHDPAT on startle amplitude and calculated %PPI (dose of 8-OHDPAT as a factor), the results of binding and in situ studies (corticosterone treatment as a factor). Two-way ANOVA was used to analyze: (1) the effect of WAY 100135 on 8-OHDPAT-induced disruption of PPI (dose of WAY and dose of 8-OHDPAT as factors), (2) the effect of corticosterone on 8-OHDPAT-induced disruption of PPI (dose of corticosterone and dose of 8-OHDPAT as factors), (3) the effect of corticosterone on 8-OHDPAT-induced changes in the level of 5-HT and 5-HIAA (dose of corticosterone and dose of 8-OHDPAT as factors). Two-way ANOVA for repeated measurements was used to analyze the effects of corticosterone on 8-OHDPAT-induced disruption on PPI calculated from row data (the effect on the amplitude of pulse without and with prepulse), doses of corticosterone and 8-OHDPAT were considered as between-group factors and amplitude of pulse vs amplitude of prepulse+pulse as a within-subject factor. When significant main effects or interactions were revealed in ANOVA, post hoc comparisons were carried out with Newman–Keuls test. p<0.05 was accepted as significant.

RESULTS

Functional Studies

Serum corticosterone levels

Corticosterone levels in blood of animals that received different corticosterone treatments are shown in Table 1. Prolonged treatment with corticosterone significantly decreased the weight of adrenal glands and body weight (Table 1).

Table 1 Effect of Corticosterone on Body Weight and Plasma Corticosterone Levels

The effect of 8-OHDPAT on startle amplitude and PPI

The 5-HT1A receptor agonist, 8-OHDPAT (1.25 and 5 mg/kg,) increased the startle amplitude and decreased PPI (Table 2). The dose of 0.2 mg/kg had no effect on either of these measures (Table 2).

Table 2 Effect of 8-OHDPAT on the Startle Reaction and PPI in Rats

The 5-HT1A receptor antagonist WAY 100135 (2.5–20 mg/kg), dose dependently attenuated the effects of 8-OHDPAT (5 mg/kg) on startle amplitude (F(1,88)=10.7, p<002) and PPI (F(1,88)=7.31, p<0.01). The dose of 5 mg/kg of WAY 100135 completely blocked the effects of 8-OHDPAT, similarly as its dose of 20 mg/kg. All used doses of WAY 100135 did not affect startle amplitude and PPI in vehicle-treated animals (Table 3).

Table 3 Effect of WAY 100135 on the 8-OHDPAT (5 mg/kg s.c.)-Evoked Disruption of PPI of the Acoustic Startle Response in Rats

The effect of corticosterone on 8-OHDPAT-induced inhibition of PPI

As shown in Figure 1a, acute corticosterone treatment had no effect on 8-OHDPAT-induced deficits in sensorimotor gating (amplitude, F(2,66)=1.30, p<0.28; PPI, F(2,66)=0.44, p<0.64), nor on these measures in control rats (amplitude F(1,66)=0.206, p<0.15; PPI, F(1,66)=0.045, p<0.83). 8-OHDPAT (1.25 and 5 mg/kg) significantly decreased PPI (F(2,66)=11.11, p<0.001), which resulted from the effect of 8-OHDPAT on the amplitude of startle reflex evoked by acoustic pulse preceded by prepulse (PP+P).

Figure 1
figure 1

Effect of corticosterone (Cor) on the amplitude of startle reflex and on the calculated %PPI induced by 8-OHDPAT (1.25 and 5 mg/kg s.c.). Experiments were performed (a) 2 h after a single dose of Cor (10 mg/kg s.c.) or (b) 2 h and (C) 24 h after the last dose of chronic treatment (Cor, 10 mg/kg s.c., twice daily, for 7 days). 8-OHDPAT was given 15 min before the test. Left and central panels illustrate the effect of 8-OHDPAT on the amplitude of startle reflex evoked by acoustic pulse (P) and acoustic pulse preceded by prepulse (PP+P) in vehicle (Veh)- and Cor-treated animals, respectively. Right panel illustrates the effect of Cor on 8-OHDPAT-evoked disruption of PPI. The values are the mean±SEM of 12 rats/group. #Significant effect on P; *significant effect on PP+P; significant effect of corticosterone on P; &significant difference between P and P+PP; ×significant effect of 8-OHDPAT on %PPI; °significant effect of corticosterone on %PPI in 8-OHDPAT-treated rats.

As shown in Figure 1b, chronic corticosterone treatment (when measured 2 h after the last dose) had no effect on PPI in vehicle- and 8-OHDPAT-treated rats (F(1,66)=1.944, p<0.17 and F(2,66)=0.255, p<0.77, respectively); however, corticosterone treatment significantly decreased the amplitude of startle reaction (F(1,66)=11.039, p<0.001) in vehicle- but not 8-OHDPAT-treated rats. 8-OHDPAT (1.25 and 5 mg/kg) significantly decreased PPI (F(2,66)=16.62, p<0.001), which resulted from the effect of 8-OHDPAT, on the amplitude of (PP+P).

As shown in Figure 1c, chronic corticosterone treatment had no effect on PPI in vehicle-treated rats (F(2,66)=1.184, p<0.28) but attenuated 8-OHDPAT-induced deficits in PPI (F(2,66)=3.314, p<0.04), when the test was performed 24 h after the last dose. Analysis of raw data (amplitude of acoustic pulse without and with prepulse) revealed significant interaction between corticosterone treatment, effects of 8-OHDPAT, and type of the applied acoustic stimuli (F(2.66)=4.923, p<0.01). 8-OHDPAT (1.25 and 5 mg/kg) significantly decreased PPI (F(2,66)=16.62, p<0.001), which resulted from the effect of 8-OHDPAT on the amplitude of (PP+P) and this effect was attenuated by corticosterone (F(1,66)=9.18, p<0.01). In this experimental design, corticosterone induced significant difference between an amplitude of the response to pulse vs an amplitude of the response to pulse preceded by prepulse in 8-OHDPAT-treated rats.

The effect of corticosterone on 8-OHDPAT-induced changes in 5-HT and 5-HIAA concentrations

In the hippocampus, single injection of 8-OHDPAT (1.25 mg/kg) did not influence 5-HT and 5-HIAA tissue levels, while chronic corticosterone treatment increased the levels of both 5-HT (F(1,40)=2.94, p<0.045) and its metabolite5-HIAA (F(1,40)=10.46, p<0.0001). After the combined treatment, 5-HT level was still increased whereas the level of 5-HIAA was significantly decreased as compared to vehicle- and corticosterone-treated animals (Figure 2). 8-OHDPAT decreased significantly ((F(1,41)=9.02; p<0.0001) 5-HIAA concentration being without effect on 5-HT level in the raphe nuclei. Chronic corticosterone administration evoked similar effect (a decrease in the level of 5-HIAA without any changes in 5-HT), whereas the combined treatment with corticosterone and 8-OHDPAT did not induce greater decreases in 5-HIAA levels than those caused by either drug alone (Figure 2). The effect of 8-OHDPAT on 5-HT metabolism manifested itself also as the decreased 5-HIAA/5-HT ratio observed both in the raphe nuclei (1.32±0.07 vs 0.98±0.05* in vehicle- and 8-OHDPAT-treated group, respectively) and in the hippocampus (1.06±0.05 vs 0.81±0.04*, respectively). Corticosterone did not influence 5-HIAA/5-HT ratio both in the raphe nuclei (1.16±0.11 and 0.81±0.04 in vehicle- and 8-OHDPAT-treated rats, respectively) and in the hippocampus (1.04±0.08 and 0.72±0.04 in vehicle- and 8-OHDPAT-treated rats).

Figure 2
figure 2

Effect of corticosterone (Cor) on the levels of 5-HT and 5-HIAA in the hippocampus and raphe nuclei of vehicle- or 8-OHDPAT-treated rats. Cor was given chronically (10 mg/kg s.c., twice daily for 7 days) and 8-OHDPAT (1.25 mg/kg, s.c.) was given 23 h after the last corticosterone dose. The animals were killed 1 h after 8-OHDPAT or respective vehicle treatment. The values are the mean+SEM of 8–16 determinations per group. Asterisks indicate statistical differences in concentrations of 5-HT and 5-HIAA, between vehicle+vehicle- and respective drug-treated rats (*p<0.05).

Receptor Studies

The effect of corticosterone on 5-HT1A receptor binding

The high levels of 5-HT1A binding sites labeled with [3H]8-OHDPAT were found in the dorsal raphe nucleus, entorhinal cortex, and hippocampus (Figure 3a, d). The effects of corticosterone on binding at 5-HT1A receptors are shown in Table 4. As revealed by a statistical analysis, the effects of corticosterone on 5-HT1A receptor binding were time- and brain region-specific. Corticosterone independent of the time of treatment did not modify the [3H]8-OHDPAT binding in the raphe nuclei. However, prolonged corticosterone treatment induced significant decreases (23–37%) in the [3H]8-OHDPAT binding in the entorhinal cortex. In the hippocampus, single dose of corticosterone did not influence binding to 5-HT1A receptors in the CA1 region but increased (16–27%) this binding in the dentate gyrus, and in the CA3 and CA4 regions. Prolonged treatment with corticosterone, decreased 5-HT1A receptor binding in the dentate gyrus (18–25%), and in the ventral part of the CA1 region (17–20%), while it was without effect in the dorsal part of the CA1 region.

Figure 3
figure 3

Representative distribution of 5-HT1A receptors and their mRNA in the rat brain sections. Dark-field film autoradiographs of [3H]8-OHDPAT (1.5 nM) binding to 5-HT1A receptors at a level of the hippocampus (a) and raphe nuclei (d). Dark-field film autoradiographs of 5-HT1A receptor mRNA, determined by in situ hybridization histochemistry at a level of the hippocampus (b) and dorsal raphe (e). Bright-field photomicrograph (objective 40 ×) of the hippocampus (c) and dorsal raphe (f), showing silver grains corresponding to the 5-HT1A receptor mRNA that overlie cell bodies counterstained with cresyl violet. For abbreviations see section Abbreviations on Table 4.

Table 4 Effect of Corticosterone on the Specific Binding of [3H]8-OHDPAT (1.5 nM) to 5-HT1A Receptors in the Rat Brain

The effect of corticosterone on 5-HT1A receptor mRNA

Figure 3b, c, e, f shows the distribution of 5-HT1A receptor mRNA within the hippocampus, dorsal raphe nucleus, and entorhinal cortex. An analysis of the autoradiographs revealed that in the dorsal raphe nucleus and entorhinal cortex, corticosterone did not induce any change in 5HT1A receptor mRNA (Table 5) while in the hippocampus only acute corticosterone treatment decreased significantly (18–19%) the expression of 5HT1A receptor mRNA in the dentate gyrus and CA1 region (Table 5).

Table 5 Effect of Corticosterone on the 5-HT1A Receptor mRNA Expression in the Rat Brain

DISCUSSION

Our experiments revealed that neither prolonged nor acute treatment with corticosterone was able to influence the PPI in control animals. However, prolonged treatment with corticosterone, which produced persistent elevation of its blood levels comparable to those observed in severe to moderate stress (de Kloet et al, 1990; Raghupathi and McGonigle, 1997), attenuated the disruptive effects of 8-OHDPAT on PPI, which may suggest that corticosterone attenuated the function of 5-HT1A receptors. The effect of 8-OHDPAT on the PPI resulted from stimulation of 5-HT1A receptors (see also Sipes and Geyer, 1995) since it was completely abolished by the dose of WAY 100135 (5 mg/kg) considered to be selective for blocking 5-HT1A receptors (Cliffe et al, 1993; Assie and Koek, 1996).

Further, we found that corticosterone given repeatedly decreased the amplitude of startle reaction, and this effect was negatively correlated with blood hormone levels. It was observed only shortly (2 h) after the last dose of prolonged treatment (24 h after the treatment the effect was no longer observed). Similar results were obtained by Glowa et al (1992) who also found an inverse relation between the startle amplitude and circulating corticosterone levels. However, it seems that corticosterone-induced attenuation of the disruptive effects of 8-OHDPAT on PPI did not result from its effects on startle, since the effects of corticosterone on startle amplitude were seen only in vehicle- and not 8-OHDPAT-treated animals. Moreover, the attenuation of the disruptive effects of 8-OHDPAT on PPI was observed at the time when the effect of corticosterone on startle amplitude already disappeared (24 h after last dose).

The mechanism by which corticosterone attenuated the responsiveness to 8-OHDPAT in PPI test is not known. As it was shown by Sipes and Geyer (1995), 8-OHDPAT injected directly into the raphe nuclei diminished PPI, which suggested that the somatodendritic 5-HT1A autoreceptors and the ability of 8-OHDPAT to decrease the activity of serotonergic neurons (Hjorth and Sharp, 1991; Sipes and Geyer, 1995; Fletcher et al, 2001; Prinssen et al, 2002) in raphe nuclei played a role in the 8-OHDPAT-induced disruption of PPI. The above arguments might have indicated that corticosterone-induced changes in 5-HT metabolism had contributed to this effect. However, we found that in the raphe nuclei both chronic treatment with corticosterone and injection of 8-OHDPAT induced decreases in 5-HT metabolism (a decrease in 5-HIAA level without changes in 5-HT level), whereas 8-OHDPAT did not induce further decreases in corticosterone-treated animals. In the hippocampus, corticosterone increased metabolism of 5-HT, elevating both the level of 5-HT and 5-HIAA, which is in line with the findings showing that glucocorticoids increase the activity of tryptophan hydroxylase, a rate-limiting enzyme in 5-HT synthesis (Singh et al, 1990) in a tissue-specific manner (Clark and Russo, 1997). 8-OHDPAT did not influence 5-HT and 5-HIAA levels in the hippocampus; however, it significantly decreased 5-HIAA/5-HT ratio (considered to reflect the decreased 5-HT turnover), and this effect of 8-OHDPAT was not changed by corticosterone. All these data together show that both corticosterone and 8-OHDPAT influence 5-HT metabolism; however, the effects of 8-OHDPAT were not attenuated by corticosterone; moreover, both drugs induced similar effects on 5-HT activity in the raphe nuclei. Therefore, the effect of corticosterone on 5-HT metabolism and somatodendritic autoreceptors is not likely to be engaged in the observed corticosterone-induced attenuation of the behavioral effects of 8-OHDPAT on PPI. This conclusion was supported further by receptor studies that showed no effect of corticosterone on 5-HT1A receptor binding and expression in the raphe nuclei.

However, we found that chronic treatment with corticosterone decreased 5-HT1A receptor binding in the dentate gyrus and in the ventral part of CA1 region of the hippocampus, as well as in the entorhinal cortex. Interestingly, these brain structures have been suggested to be implicated in the regulation of PPI (Bast et al, 2001; Swerdlow et al, 2001a,2001b). It is clear from our previous studies that the 5-HT1A receptors are important for the modulation of PPI disrupted not only by agonists of these receptors but also by NMDA receptor antagonists (Wedzony et al, 1997,2000). The detrimental effect of MK-801 on PPI was blocked by 5-HT1A receptor antagonist WAY 100135 (Wedzony et al, 2000). Moreover, MK-801 used at doses that disrupted PPI in rats increased 5-HT1A receptor binding, and this effect was especially potent and long lasting in the entorhinal cortex and ventral part of the hippocampus (Wedzony et al, 1997). Thus, it seems reasonable to suggest that region-specific decreases in 5-HT1A receptor binding may contribute to the corticosterone-induced attenuation of effects of 8-OHDPAT on PPI. Azmitia et al (1996) have shown that, in the hippocampus and cerebral cortex, the 5-HT1A receptor protein is located on initial segments of axons of pyramidal cells; therefore, these receptors are in strategic position for the regulation of cell excitability and output signal propagation. In line with this assumption, we observed decreased electrophysiological responsiveness to 8-OHPAT in CA1 hippocampal neurons of the corticosterone-treated rats (Czyrak et al, 2002).

Corticosterone induced downregulation of the 5-HT1A receptors after prolonged, but not after single, administration, whereas only single injection of corticosterone was capable of decreasing the 5-HT1A receptor mRNA in the hippocampus. These results suggest that acute changes in plasma corticosterone are very rapidly accompanied by decreases in 5-HT1A receptor mRNA, whereas alterations in the receptor protein levels occur much more slowly. That is not surprising in the light of the results (Pinto and Battaglia, 1994) showing that a half-life of the 5-HT1A receptor protein is ca 5 days. Our results, presenting decreased expression of 5-HT1A receptors after corticosterone treatment, are generally in line with many other studies (Mendelson and McEwen, 1992a,1992b; Meijer et al, 1997; Takao et al, 1997; Lopez et al, 1998,1999; Karten et al, 1999), although the lack of changes particularly in the CA1 region of hippocampus was also reported (Mendelson and McEwen, 1992a,1992b; Holmes et al, 1995; Fernandes et al, 1997). This inconsistency may be explained by our observation that the effect of corticosterone on 5-HT1A receptor binding differs from dorsal to ventral part of CA1 region, whereas the studies cited above did not differentiate parts of this region.

Behavioral responsiveness to 8-OHDPAT was attenuated in rats that received prolonged, but not acute, treatment with corticosterone, which suggests that the effect of corticosterone developed with time as an adaptive decrease in 5-HT1A receptor functioning. The effects of corticosterone on hyperpolarization induced by 8-OHDPAT in the hippocampus (Bijak et al, 2001; Czyrak et al, 2002) as well as on cyclic AMP generating system (Czyrak, 1996), to which 5-HT1A receptor is negatively coupled, were also observed only after prolonged treatment and persisted up to 48 h after the treatment. Correspondingly, the decreases in 5-HT1A receptor binding were also observed after chronic treatment with corticosterone. However, receptor changes were observed both 2 and 24 h after the last treatment, whereas functional changes appeared 24 h after the last dose. This discrepancy is difficult to interpret. Possibly, the high corticosterone level reached after hormone injection influenced not only 5-HT1A receptor binding but also other intracellular processes (G proteins, regulators of G protein signaling, potassium channels) distal to receptor protein (Okuhara et al, 1997; Ni et al, 1999; Muma and Beck, 1999; Dwivedi and Pandey, 2000) giving the above final behavioral effect.

Summing up, we have shown that prolonged rise in corticosterone level induced functional desensitization of 5-HT1A receptors measured as an attenuation of 8-OHDPAT-induced disruption of PPI, which was paralleled by the downregulation of the 5-HT1A receptors in the brain structures (ventral hippocampus and entorhinal cortex) shown to be engaged in the regulation of PPI. The results suggest that region-specific changes in 5-HT1A receptor density may be responsible for the observed desensitization of the effects of 8-OHDPAT. Although the corticosterone-evoked attenuation of the effects of 8-OHDPAT on PPI indicates general functional downregulation of 5-HT1A receptors rather than modulation of any specific deficit, a question arises whether this apparent beneficial effect of corticosterone might reflect its putative effectiveness in preventing at least some symptoms of illnesses during which sensorimotor gating is disordered.

NOTE TO READERS

It is with deep sorrow that we would like to inform you that Anna Czyrak passed away during the final stages of sending this paper to press. Her unexpected death is a painful and irreparable loss for us. Her passing has left us bereft of an open-hearted, open-minded person, distinguished scientist and our unforgettable friend.