Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current Issue
    • Fast Forward
    • Latest Articles
    • Archive
  • Information
    • Instructions to Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • For Subscribers
    • For Advertisers
    • Terms & Conditions of Use
    • Permissions
  • Editorial Board
  • Alerts
    • Alerts
    • RSS Feeds
  • Virtual Issues
  • Feedback
  • Other Publications
    • Drug Metabolism and Disposition
    • Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
    • Molecular Pharmacology
    • Pharmacological Reviews
    • Pharmacology Research & Perspectives
    • ASPET

User menu

  • My alerts
  • Log in
  • Log out

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
  • Other Publications
    • Drug Metabolism and Disposition
    • Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
    • Molecular Pharmacology
    • Pharmacological Reviews
    • Pharmacology Research & Perspectives
    • ASPET
  • My alerts
  • Log in
  • Log out
Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Articles
    • Current Issue
    • Fast Forward
    • Latest Articles
    • Archive
  • Information
    • Instructions to Authors
    • Submit a Manuscript
    • For Subscribers
    • For Advertisers
    • Terms & Conditions of Use
    • Permissions
  • Editorial Board
  • Alerts
    • Alerts
    • RSS Feeds
  • Virtual Issues
  • Feedback
  • Visit jpet on Facebook
  • Follow jpet on Twitter
  • Follow jpet on LinkedIn
Research ArticleBEHAVIORAL PHARMACOLOGY

Acquisition, Expression, and Reinstatement of Ethanol-Induced Conditioned Place Preference in Mice: Effects of Opioid Receptor-Like 1 Receptor Agonists and Naloxone

A. Kuzmin, J. Sandin, L. Terenius and S. O. Ögren
Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics January 2003, 304 (1) 310-318; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1124/jpet.102.041350
A. Kuzmin
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
J. Sandin
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
L. Terenius
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
S. O. Ögren
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

The ability of the two opioid receptor-like receptor 1 (ORL1) agonists nociceptin (5 nmol i.c.v.) and synthetic (1S,3aS)-8-(2,3,3a,4,5,6-hexahydro-1H-phenalen-1-yl)-1-phenyl-1,3,8-triaza-spiro[4.5]decan-4-one hydrochloride (Ro 64-6198; 0.1, 0.3, and 1.0 mg/kg i.p.) and the opioid antagonist naloxone (0.1, 1.0, and 10.0 mg/kg s.c.) to modify ethanol-induced conditioned place preference was examined in NMRI male mice. The ORL1 agonists were found to significantly reduce the acquisition, expression, and ethanol-induced reinstatement of conditioned place preference. Unlike the ORL1 agonists, naloxone at the doses relevant for opioid receptor blockade failed to significantly influence the acquisition of ethanol-induced conditioned place preference. However, naloxone at 1.0 but not 0.1 mg/kg s.c. potently blocked the expression of ethanol-induced conditioned place preference and significantly inhibited ethanol-induced reinstatement of the conditioned place preference after extinction. Separate experiments indicated that nociceptin and Ro 64-6198 are both devoid of reinforcing or aversive properties. Naloxone, however, at 1.0 and 10.0 mg/kg, produced conditioned place aversion, indicating motivational properties of its own. Both nociceptin and Ro 64-6198 reduced locomotor activity after acute administration. However, tolerance developed very quickly to this effect and already after three i.c.v. (or i.p.) injections, there was no significant reduction of locomotor activity. It is concluded that ORL1 agonists can modulate the acquisition, expression, and reinstatement of the conditioned reinforcing effects of ethanol with no reinforcing or aversive properties of their own. This property might be a potential advantage in the treatment of alcoholism compared with nonselective opioid antagonist naltrexone.

Relapse to alcohol use after periods of abstinence is a common feature of alcoholism. Animal models have contributed to the identification of factors that may underlie the motivational aspects of ethanol and that contribute to relapse to ethanol taking. Drug-seeking behavior for ethanol can be initiated or maintained not only by the primary, reinforcing effects of the drugs themselves but also by secondary, drug-associated stimuli (for review, see O'Brien, 1975). The most common experimental model used to study relapse to drug seeking has been the reinstatement procedure. After training to make a response to self-administer a drug and the subsequent extinction of the acquired response, an acute noncontingent injection of test drug (or acute stress) results in the reinstatement of responding (Carroll and Comer, 1996). The conditioned place preference (CPP) procedure provides an alternative experimental approach to examine mechanisms underlying relapse. In this procedure a particular stimulus context or test environment is paired with the effects of the drug, without the animal having to learn to make an explicit response to obtain the drug. In the test trial, the animal is allowed to freely move between the test environment previously paired with the drug administration and the nondrug environment. By its behavior the animal can show which environment it prefers (for review, see Tzschentke, 1998). Recently, this learning paradigm was used to assess the relapse effect of a “priming” injection of a drug (e.g., cocaine; Mueller and Stewart, 2000). It was shown that a priming injection of the drug used to induce the conditioned place preference, given after extinction conditions, restored the salience or attractiveness of the environment previously paired with drug administration.

The neurochemical substrates for relapse to ethanol have been studied at some length. There is now evidence for a role of the opioid systems in the risk for relapse due to their ability to mediate alcohol-induced euphoria. Naltrexone, a nonselective opioid antagonist, has a proven potential as a therapeutic agent for alcoholism. Naltrexone and its short-lasting analog naloxone display “antialcohol” properties in several behavioral paradigms (for review, see Froehlich and Li, 1994).

Recent studies have also focused attention to the brain nociceptin system in relation to the rewarding effects of ethanol (Ciccocioppo et al., 1999, 2000; Martin-Fardon et al., 2000). Nociceptin, a 17-amino acid neuropeptide, was found to be a natural ligand of the G protein-coupled opioid receptor-like 1 receptor (ORL1) (Meunier et al., 1995; Reinscheid et al., 1995). Despite the structural similarity with dynorphin-A, nociceptin does not bind to opioid receptors and its pharmacological effects are not sensitive to naloxone treatment (Jenck et al., 2000). In general, nociceptin has an inhibitory role on synaptic transmission in the central nervous system and thereby may reduce the responsiveness to external (e.g., stress) or pharmacological (e.g., ethanol) stimuli (Calo et al., 2000). The peptide has also been reported to inhibit the activation of antinociceptive neurons in the nucleus raphe magnus and periaqueductal gray as well as hypothalamic β-endorphinergic neurons, projecting to the ventral tegmental area and nucleus accumbens (Wagner et al., 1998). Nociceptin, injected i.c.v., was found to modify ethanol intake in rats and to prevent the acquisition of ethanol-induced conditioned place preference in rats (Ciccocioppo et al., 1999). The peptide also had an inhibitory effect on ethanol-seeking behavior in rats trained for ethanol self-administration after exposure to foot shock (Ciccocioppo et al., 2000), and it significantly reduced the effects of foot shock stress on ethanol-seeking behavior (Martin-Fardon et al., 2000). In place conditioning experiments in rats, nociceptin did not affect place preference, suggesting that it is devoid of “motivational” properties of its own (Devine et al., 1996a; Ciccocioppo et al., 1999).

Studies on the functional role of the nociceptin receptor (ORL1) have been hampered by the lack of potent and selective ligands. Recently, the nonpeptide compound (1S,3aS)-8-(2,3,3a,4,5,6-hexahydro-1H-phenalen-1-yl)-1-phenyl-1,3,8-triaza-spiro[4.5]decan-4-one hydrochloride (mol.wt. 438.017) (Ro 64-6198), which penetrates well to brain after i.p. administration, has been described (Jenck et al., 2000; Wichmann et al., 2000). Ro 64-6198 was shown to be a full agonist with an affinity to the ORL1 receptor close to that of nociceptin itself (Jenck et al., 2000).

The aim of the present work is to investigate the effect of nociceptin and Ro 64-6198 on the acquisition, expression, and reinstatement of ethanol-induced conditioned place preference in mice and to compare their effects with those of naloxone (a nonselective opioid antagonist). In addition, the role of endogenous nociceptin for the effects of ethanol in the same experimental paradigm was studied in pronociceptin gene knockout mice.

Materials and Methods

Animals

Experiments with naloxone, nociceptin, and Ro 64-6198 were carried out in male NMRI mice (20–25 g; B&K Universal, AB, Sweden). Animals were kept under standard laboratory conditions with free access to food pellets (low-protein diet) and tap water. The animals were housed four to six per cage in a light-controlled room (12-h light/dark cycle, light on at 6:00 AM) at 21°C and 60% humidity. The experiments were approved by the Northern Stockholm Ethics Board of Animal Experimentation. All experiments were performed between 10:00 AM and 1:00 PM.

Nociceptin knockout mice were obtained from Dr. F. Jenck (Roche CNS Research, Basel, Switzerland). Briefly, a null mutation was introduced into the coding region of the OFQ peptide of the mouse ppOFQ gene by homologous recombination in embryonic stem cells of 129/Ola mice (Koster et al., 1999). Two clones were selected in which the correct disruption at one ppOFQ allele was confirmed and were injected into blastocysts of C57BL/6J embryos. Two male chimeric mice transmitted the mutated allele to their offspring and were mated with C57BL/6J female mice. Mice heterozygous for the ppOFQ mutation seemed normal, healthy, and fertile for at least 12 months on the mixed 129/Ola-C57BL/6J genetic background. Heterozygous mice (±) were intercrossed and the offspring were genotyped by Southern blot or polymerase chain reaction. Animals have not been backcrossed into a pure genetic background but +/+ and −/− littermates of the F2 or F3 generation with mixed 129-C57 background were cumulated in groups. Data comparing male −/− and male +/+ mice are presented.

Surgery and Cannulation

The mice were anesthetized with a combination of Ketalar (50 mg/ml ketamine hydrochloride, 1:10, 0.2 ml s.c.; Parke Davis, Barcelona, Spain) and Hypnorm (10 mg/ml fluanisonum + 0.2 mg/ml fentanylum; 1:10, 0.2 ml i.p.; Janssen, Beerse, Belgium). The body temperature was maintained at 37°C using a thermostat-regulated heat pad (CMA/105; CMA/Microdialysis, Stockholm, Sweden). Permanent steel guide cannulae with an outer diameter of 0.4 mm (Plastics One, Roanoke, VA) were implanted into the right lateral ventricle using coordinates based on the stereotaxic plates [AP (bregma), −2.5 mm; L, 3 mm; V, 4.25 mm; Franklin and Paxinos, 1997). Cannulae were fixed to the scull by dental carboxylic cement (Durelon ESPE; Sollentuna, Sweden). The animals were allowed to recover for 2 to 3 days in the colony room (2–3 mice/cage) before the start of the experiment.

Drugs

Naloxone HCl (EndoLab, Wilmington, PA) was dissolved in saline and injected s.c. in a 5-ml/kg volume. Nociceptin (1–17, peptide free base, mol.wt. 1809; Tocris, Bristol, UK) was dissolved in artificial CSF just before injection. Artificial mice CSF contained 7.20 g/l NaCl, 1.96 g/l NaHCO3, 0.18 g/l KCl, 0.068 g/l KH2PO4, 0.16 g/l CaCl2, 0.17 g/l MgCl2 · 6H2O, 0.07 g/l Na2SO4, and 1.0 g/l glucose. The pH of the solution was adjusted to 7.1 with 0.01 N NaOH and HCl. After i.c.v. injection, the needle remained in place for 3 to 5 s.

Ro 64-6198 was a gift of Dr. Juergen Wichmann (F. Hoffmann-La Roche, Pharma Preclinical Research, Basel, Switzerland). The drug was freshly dissolved in saline and was administered i.p. (5-ml/kg injection volume).

Ethanol solutions (ethanol 95%, density = 0.89) were prepared with tap water (v/v) to the required concentration and were administered intragastrically through a specially designed Teflon-coated needle (AgnTho's, Lidingo, Sweden) in a volume 0.2 ml/mouse.

Conditioned Place Preference

Apparatus.

Place preference experiments were performed in plastic shuttle boxes divided by guillotine doors into two compartments of equal size (25 × 25 × 25 cm) with different degrees of illumination (30 and 110 lux) and color (black and white, respectively). Floor texture (solid) was equal in both compartments. The location of the animal in the box was monitored with a video camera-based computerized imaging system (Videomot; TSE GmbH, Homburg, Germany).

Experimental Design.

Each experiment consisted of a preconditioning, conditioning, and postconditioning phase (Fig.1). During the preconditioning phase mice were placed in the center of the white compartment facing the opened door between the compartments. This 10-min procedure was repeated 1 h later. Time spent in the white compartment during the second preconditioning session was defined as the baseline level of preference. The experimental conditions were chosen (see above) to result in a balanced preference for both compartments during the preconditioning test. In addition, on the basis of the results from preconditioning test we excluded from the experiments mice with a tendency for unbalanced preference (more than 60% of time in one of the compartments). Although the criteria for exclusion were relatively strict, most of the mice were found to fulfill them (60–80% depending on the group).

Figure 1
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1

Experimental design and time schedule for the conditioned place preference experiments. In the preconditioning phase (pretest) mice were placed two times in the shuttle boxes with open doors for 10 min, the interval between placing was no less than 1 h. Time spent in the white compartment during the second preconditioning session was considered as the baseline level of preference. The conditioning phase consisted of two 20-min daily sessions on four consecutive days. On each of the 4 days of conditioning mice received water i.g. (5 min before the first daily conditioning session) and placed randomly to one of the compartments for 20 min. Thereupon, they received an i.g. ethanol solution and were placed into the opposite compartment for 20 min with the door between compartments closed. The control group received water i.g. in both compartments with the random starting placement (i.e., half of mice in each group started from the white compartment and half from the black). After the completion of two 20-min trials, the animals were returned to their home cages. The postconditioning (post-test) took place 48 h after the last conditioning session. The mice in a drug-free state (except in experiments with the expression of CPP) were placed in the apparatus with open doors for 10 min, and the time spent in the white compartment and the number of crossings between compartments were automatically recorded in the same manner as during the preconditioning test. The difference of time spent in the ethanol-associated compartment during post- versus preconditioning test was taken as a measure of drug-induced place preference. In the reinstatement protocol after postconditioning (post-test 1) the extinction trials (4 days) were performed. On each day mice were allowed to investigate freely both compartments for 20 min. During the last extinction trial the preference for the compartments was recorded again (last 10 min of the trial, post-test 2). The following day, mice were operated and the cannulae were implanted into the lateral ventricles. Three days after the operation (mice were left undisturbed) the reinstatement experiment was performed. Mice were treated i.g. either with 5% ethanol solution (0.2 ml/animals) or with water and were allowed to explore the shuttle boxes for 10 min (post-test 3). The time spent in the white compartment and the number of crossings between compartments were recorded as described above.

The conditioning phase consisted of two 20-min daily sessions on four consecutive days (total eight sessions: four with ethanol and four with water). On each of the four days of conditioning mice received water i.g. (5 min before the first daily conditioning session) and placed randomly to one of the compartments for 20 min. Thereupon, they received an i.g. ethanol solution (5, 10, and 20%, 0.2 ml/mouse, corresponding to 0.4, 0.8, and 1.6 g/kg ethanol) and were placed into the opposite compartment for 20 min with the door between compartments closed. The control group received water i.g. in both compartments with the random starting placement (i.e., half of mice in each group started from white compartment and half from black). After the completion of two 20-min trials, the animals were returned to their home cages. To avoid possible influence of intragastric food on ethanol absorption, mice were food deprived 1 h before the conditioning procedure.

The postconditioning test was conducted 48 h after the last conditioning session. During the postconditioning test, mice in a drug-free state (except in the experiments with the expression of conditioned place preference) were placed in the apparatus with open doors for 10 min and the time spent in the white compartment and the number of crossings between compartments were automatically recorded in the same manner as during the preconditioning test. Due to the degree of illumination the video tracking system detected mice only in the white compartment. Time spent in the opposite black part was calculated as a difference between the total time of the experiment and the time spent in the white compartment. The difference of time spent in the ethanol-associated compartment during post- versus preconditioning test was taken as the measure of drug-induced place preference. In control experiments (conditioning with water in both compartments) the shift of time was calculated with reference to the compartment used for the second conditioning with water. The boxes were cleaned and deodorized with 3% H2O2 solution after each animal.

Effects of Nociceptin/Ro 64-6198/Naloxone on the Acquisition of Ethanol-Induced Place Conditioning.

The experiments were performed as described above but during the conditioning period mice were pretreated 2 min before the second conditioning trial (0.8 g/kg ethanol or water administration) with nociceptin (5 nmol in 2 μl/mouse i.c.v.) or Ro 64-6198 (0.3, 1.0, and 3.0 mg/kg i.p.) or naloxone (0.1, 1.0, and 10.0 mg/kg s.c.). Animals in the control group received i.c.v. injections of artificial cerebrospinal fluid (2 μl) or i.p./s.c. injection of saline, respectively.

Effects of Nociceptin/Ro 64-6198/Naloxone on the Expression of Ethanol-Induced Place Conditioning.

The experiments were performed as described above (conditioning with 0.8-g/kg ethanol dose). Nociceptin (5 nmol/2 μl i.c.v.) or Ro 64-6198 (0.3, 1.0, and 3.0 mg/kg i.p.) or naloxone (0.1, 1.0, and 10.0 mg/kg s.c.) were injected only once just before the postconditioning test. Animals in the control group received i.c.v. injections of artificial cerebrospinal fluid (2 μl) or i.p./s.c. injection of saline, respectively.

Influence of Nociceptin/Ro 64-6198/Naloxone on the Reinstatement of Ethanol-Induced Place Preference by a Priming Injection of Ethanol Solution.

The acquisition of ethanol-induced (0.8-g/kg ethanol dose) place preference was performed as already described. After being tested for place preference, mice were subjected to 4 days of extinction trials (one trial per day) during which the mice were allowed to investigate both compartments for 20 min. During the last extinction trial the preference for the compartments was again recorded (last 10 min of the trial).

For experiments with nociceptin i.c.v. administration, the day after the last extinction trial the mice were operated and cannulae were implanted into the lateral ventricles. Three days after the operation (mice were left undisturbed) the reinstatement experiment was initiated. Mice were treated i.g. with either a 0.4-g/kg ethanol dose or with water. Five minutes after ethanol/water administration the mice were treated with nociceptin (5 nmol/2 μl i.c.v.) or CSF (2 μl i.c.v.) and allowed to explore the shuttle boxes. The time spent in the white compartment and the number of crossings between compartments were recorded as described above.

The experiments with Ro 64-6198/naloxone administration were run similarly to the nociceptin study, e.g., 3 days after the last extinction trial. In the reinstatement experiment, separate groups of mice received, 5 min after being treated i.g. with either ethanol (0.4 g/kg) or with water, Ro 64-6198 (0.3 mg/kg i.p.), naloxone (1.0 mg/kg s.c.), or saline (i.p.) and were then allowed to explore the shuttle boxes. The time spent in the ethanol-associated compartment and the number of crossings between compartments were recorded as described above.

Statistics

Data for the place conditioning effect (shift of the time spent on the ethanol-paired side) as well as the numbers of crossings between compartments during the 10-min test were analyzed by one- or two-way ANOVA followed by post hoc Newman-Keuls test when appropriate.

Effects of Ro 64-6198 on Locomotor Activity

Mice were individually tested in a dimly lit sound-controlled area ventilated by fans. They were removed from their home cages and placed in the middle of an activity monitor [standard transparent A3 (42 × 26 × 20 cm) Macrolon cage B&K Universal, AB, Sweden with 50 ml of wood shavings on the floor] and the data-collecting system was immediately activated. In experiments with nonhabituated animals, mice were treated with the drug just before placement in the activity box and different parameters of motor activity were recorded during 6 × 10-min intervals.

Motor activity was measured in eight animals simultaneously by means of a multicage red and infrared-sensitive motion detection system (Ögren et al., 1986). The system is fully computerized and uses beams of red and infrared lights in combination with vertical (infrared light sensitive photocells in the walls of the apparatus) and horizontal (red light-sensitive photocells in the floor of the apparatus) arrays. The distance between photocells is 4 cm. Rearing was measured by counting the number of times an animal stands on its hind legs and interferes with any of the six invisible infrared beams passing horizontally through the cages. The height of these photocells was adapted to the size of the animal. Motility was measured as all movements of a distance of 4 cm or more detected by 48 vertical photocells, and represents a measurement of general activity. Locomotion was measured by counting the number of times an animal had covered eight horizontal photocells and moved from one side of the test cage to the other side (a distance of at least 32 cm).

Effects of Subchronic Treatment with Ro 64-6198 on the Locomotor Inhibitory Effect of Ro 64-6198

Drug and experimentally naı̈ve mice were treated twice a day (10:00 AM and 6:00 PM) i.p. for 4 days with either saline or 1 mg/kg Ro 64-6198. On day 5, mice were randomly divided into three treatment groups (n = 8):

1.
Subchronic treatment with saline + acute treatment with Ro 64-6198 (1 mg/kg).
2.
Subchronic treatment with Ro 64-6198 + acute treatment with Ro 64-6198 (1 mg/kg).
3.
Subchronic treatment with saline + acute treatment with saline. Immediately after treatment mice were placed into the “activity” boxes and their rearing, motility, and locomotion were recorded for 6 × 10-min periods.

Dose-dependent effects of Ro 64-6198 on rearing, motility, and locomotion in mice were analyzed by ANOVA for repeated measures, the factors being treatment (drug doses and saline) and time (6 × 10-min recording). This was followed by a post hoc Newman-Keuls test for every time period recorded. The whole study was designed as a between subjects (independent groups) experiment (i.e., each animal was used only once).

Results

Dose-Dependent Effects of Ethanol in the CPP Paradigm

Measurement of the natural preference in the preconditioning test indicated that the mice displayed no preference for the particular compartment. One-way ANOVA revealed no significant differences between the experimental groups either in time spent in the white compartment or in the number of crossings between the compartments during the 10-min experiment. The mean number of crossings was about 17 to 25 in 10 min.

In animals with water conditioning (i.g. administration before placement in both compartments), a slight tendency for an increase in the time spent in the white compartment was found. No changes in the exploratory activity (number of crossings) were found after conditioning with water in both compartments.

Ethanol produced a significant place conditioning effect in NMRI mice (Fig. 2). ANOVA revealed a significant effect with respect to the shift of time spent in the ethanol-paired compartment [F(3,20) = 6.5, p < 0.01]. Post hoc test revealed that the 0.8-g/kg ethanol group differed significantly from the water group (p < 0.01) and the mean increase in time spent in ethanol paired compartment was 95.3 s.

Figure 2
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2

Dose-related effects of ethanol in the conditioned place preference paradigm. Data (mean and S.E.M.) represent the shift of time, spent in ethanol-paired compartment in comparison with baseline preference. Numbers above the columns represent the number of crossings between compartments in the postconditioning test. ∗,p < 0.01, compared with the group conditioned with water (0).

Ethanol-Induced CPP in Nociceptin Knockout (KO) Mice

Nociceptin KO male mice and their wild-type (WT) controls were tested for the acquisition of ethanol-induced conditioned place preference comparing 0 (water), 0.4-, 0.8-, and 1.6-g/kg ethanol doses (Fig. 3). Two-way ANOVA of the shift of time spent in the ethanol-paired compartment revealed a significant ethanol dose-related effect [F(3,35) = 8.2,p < 0.01] but no genotype effect [F(1,35) = 3.0, p = 0.08] and no genotype × dose interaction [F(3,35) = 0.3,p = 0.8]. In general, no genotype differences were found but the nociceptin KO mice tended to show a stronger response to ethanol (Fig. 3A). No significant genotype differences were found with respect to the number of crossings between white and black compartments (20–25 crossings in 10-min experiment) in post-test (Fig. 3C). However, KO mice exhibited higher exploratory activity in the pretest (Fig. 3B), which was indicated by a significantly higher number of crossings between compartments [genotype effectF(1,35) = 5.2, p < 0.05]. Thus, it was concluded that KO mice in comparison with WT mice displayed higher exploratory activity in the novel environment and tended to show a higher sensitivity to the conditioning effects of ethanol.

Figure 3
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3

Dose-related effects of ethanol in nociceptin KO and control (WT) mice. A, data (mean and S.E.M.) represent the shift of time, spent in ethanol-paired compartment in comparison with baseline preference. ∗, p < 0.05 [significant shift in time compared with the corresponding control group (0)]. B, data (mean and S.E.M.) represent the number of crossings between compartments in pretest. #, p < 0.05 (significant differences between KO and WT mice). C, data (mean and S.E.M.) represent the number of crossings between compartments in post-test.

Effects of Nociceptin/Ro 64-6198 on the acquisition of ethanol-induced CPP

Nociceptin (5 nmol/2 μl/mouse) administration blocked (Fig.4A) the acquisition of ethanol-induced (0.8 g/kg) conditioned place preference [F(3,35) = 6.7, p < 0.01]. The post hoc test revealed a significant (p < 0.01) reduction in the shift of time spent in the ethanol-paired compartment in the group pretreated with nociceptin before ethanol administration. However, the groups treated with CSF and nociceptin and received conditioning with water in both compartments, did not differ. No significant reduction of the number of crossing in groups treated with nociceptin during the post-test was found.

Figure 4
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4

Influence of nociceptin (i.c.v.) and Ro 64-6198 (i.p.) on the acquisition of ethanol [10% (v/v)]-induced conditioned place preference. A, experiments with nociceptin 5 nmol/2 μl i.c.v. administration. NC, nociceptin; VEH, vehicle; ET, ethanol; WAT, water. B, experiments with Ro 64-6198 administration (0.1, 0.3, and 1.0 mg/kg i.p.). C, experiments with naloxone (0.1, 1.0, and 10.0 mg/kg s.c.). For the legends, see Fig. 2. #, in experiments with water conditioning the shift of time was calculated with reference to compartment with second water conditioning in combination with the drug treatment.

Administration of Ro 64-6198 (0.1, 0.3, and 1.0 mg/kg i.p.) reduced ethanol-induced conditioned place preference [Fig. 4B;F(3,28) = 33.5, p < 0.01] with a significant effect even at the lowest dose tested (0.1 mg/kg,p < 0.01 compared with the vehicle-treated group). It is interesting that the combined administration of ethanol and Ro 64-6198 at the 0.3-mg/kg dose produced a significant aversion for the compartment associated with the combination (−87.1 ± 9.7 s). There was a significant reduction in the number of crossings between the compartments in the group that received Ro 64-6198 at the dose of 0.3 mg/kg (p < 0.05) but not at the 1.0-mg/kg dose. In experiments with water conditioning, similar to nociceptin, Ro 64-6198 produced neither place preference nor place aversion, and there were no differences between groups in the shift of time spent in the compartment associated with drug treatment [F(3,28) = 0.11, p = 0.9].

Effects of Naloxone on the Acquisition of Ethanol-Induced CPP

Administration of naloxone (0.1, 1.0, and 10.0 mg/kg s.c.) reduced ethanol-induced conditioned place preference (Fig. 4C) in a dose-related way [F(3,20) = 9.8, p < 0.01]. However, a significant effect was found only at the highest dose tested (10 mg/kg, p < 0.01, compared with the saline-treated group). It is important to note that mice treated with the naloxone (10 mg/kg) displayed reduced spontaneous locomotor (significant reduction of the number of crossings, p < 0.01) activity, high level of grooming, and generalized reaction to external stimuli with jumping and screaming. All this indicates that naloxone at the 10-mg/kg dose causes nonspecific effects probably unrelated to opioid systems. Importantly, the combined administration of naloxone (10 mg/kg) and ethanol produced a significant aversion for the compartment associated with the combination (−41.5 ± 19.7 s). In experiments with water conditioning, naloxone at the 0.1-mg/kg dose produced neither place preference nor place aversion. However, the higher doses of naloxone (1.0 and 10 mg/kg) produced place aversion (−12.2 ± 8.5 and −40.5 ± 21.3 s, respectively) and a significant reduction of motor activity as shown previously with the 10-mg/kg dose.

Effects of Nociceptin/Ro 64-6198 on the Expression of Ethanol-Induced CPP

In this set of experiments mice received a single administration of nociceptin or Ro 64-6198 just before the postconditioning test (Fig.5). In the experiments with nociceptin administration (Fig. 5A) ANOVA revealed a significant group effect with respect to the shift of time in the ethanol-paired compartment [F(3,32) = 9.2, p < 0.01] and the number of crossings in the postconditioning test [F(3,32) = 10,4, p < 0.01]. Nociceptin administration blocked the expression of the ethanol-induced CPP and reverted it to a place aversion (mean shift of time −54.3 ± 22.1, n = 9). In contrast, administration of nociceptin to the animals, which received conditioning with water, failed to produce place aversion (mean shift of time 34.0 ± 22.2,n = 8). Nociceptin treatment also significantly reduced the number of crossings between compartments (p < 0.01) in both groups (e.g., water-conditioning group and ethanol-conditioning group).

Figure 5
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 5

Influence of nociceptin (i.c.v.) and Ro 64-6198 (i.p.) on the expression of ethanol [10% (v/v)]-induced conditioned place preference. A, experiments with nociceptin administration (5 nmol/2 μl, i.c.v.). B, experiments with Ro 64-6198 administration (0.1, 0.3, and 1.0 mg/kg i.p.). C, experiments with naloxone (0.1, 1.0, and 10.0 mg/kg s.c.). For legends, see Fig. 2. #, in experiments with water conditioning the shift of time was calculated with reference to white compartment due to the balanced preference in pretest.

Ro 64-6198 (0.1, 0.3, and 1.0 mg/kg i.p.) also blocked the expression of ethanol-induced CPP (Fig. 5B) in a dose-related way [dose effect:F(3,18) = 16.4, p < 0.01] and reversed it to a clearcut place aversion at the dose of 1 mg/kg (mean shift of time −51.7 ± 13.8, n = 8). Interestingly, the same dose of Ro 64-6198 failed to produce place aversion in the group conditioned with water (mean shift of time 15.3 ± 6.1, n = 8). Moreover, no significant differences between groups were observed [dose effect:F(3,12) = 0.48, p = 0.6]. Treatment with Ro 64-6198 (1 mg/kg) produced a significant reduction in the number of crossings between compartments in the postconditioning test.

Effects of Naloxone on the Expression of Ethanol-Induced CPP

Treatment with naloxone (0.1, 1.0, and 10.0 mg/kg s.c.; Fig. 5C) blocked the expression of ethanol-induced CPP in a dose-related way [dose effect: F(3,20) = 12.2, p < 0.01] and reversed it to a clear-cut place aversion at the dose of 10 mg/kg (mean shift of time −52.8 ± 22.7, n = 8). As in the acquisition experiments mice treated with the 10-mg/kg dose of naloxone displayed behavioral inhibition and a significant reduction of the number of crossings between compartments in post-test. Administration of the opioid antagonist to the groups, which had received conditioning with water, failed to produce significant place aversion, although some tendency for the avoidance of the white compartment was observed in mice treated with 10 mg/kg naloxone regardless of the starting compartment in conditioning trials. No significant differences between groups were observed [dose effect:F(3,20) = 0.72, p = 0.6] in mice with water conditioning.

Effect of Nociceptin, Ro 64-6198 and Naloxone on the Ethanol-Induced Reinstatement of CPP after Extinction

Figure 6 shows the results of the experiments in which animals first acquired CPP and then were given four daily extinction trials, and then received a single priming i.g. administration of ethanol (0.4 g/kg i.g.). The data were analyzed using a two-way repeated measures ANOVA with water versus ethanol conditioning as the first between group factor and drug treatment (nociceptin, CSF, saline, Ro 64-6198, and naloxone) as the second between group factor. Three data blocks (post-test 1, post-test 2, and post-test 3; Fig. 1) were used as the repeated measurement factor. ANOVA revealed a significant conditioning effect [F(1,80) = 63.9, p < 0.01] as well as a significant interaction between conditioning and treatment [F(4,80) = 2.7, p < 0.05] and conditioning and time [F(2,160) = 37.4,p < 0.01]. Treatment effect and treatment × time interaction were also significant [F(4,80) = 6.0,p < 0.01 and F(8,160) = 6.5,p < 0.01, respectively].

Figure 6
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 6

Effect of nociceptin, Ro 64-6198, and naloxone on the ethanol-induced reinstatement of the conditioned place preference after a week of extinction. Data as mean ± S.E.M. (n = 8–9). Data (mean and S.E.M.) represent the shift of time spent in ethanol paired compartment compared with baseline preference. Expression, data after conditioning procedure. Extinction, data after extinction procedure. Ethanol priming, data after priming procedure. #, in experiments with water conditioning the shift of time was calculated with reference to white compartment due to the balanced preference in pretest.

It can be seen that time spent in the ethanol-paired chamber diminished after the extinction trials (Fig. 6) and it did not differ from the time spent in the water-paired chamber. Analysis of the shift of time in the three testing blocks (e.g., acquisition, extinction, and priming) revealed a significant shift of time spent in the ethanol-paired compartment [F(2,20) = 7.62,p < 0.01] in the control groups (i.c.v. CSF or i.p. saline), which received conditioning with ethanol solution (Fig. 6). This indicates that the time spent in the ethanol-paired compartment significantly increased after acquisition and after priming with ethanol (both p < 0.01, compared with the values obtained in pretest) but did not differ from pretest values after extinction trials. In contrast, animals that received only water administration (Fig. 6), displayed no significant shift of preference (compared with pretest values) after acquisition, extinction and priming with 0.4-g/kg ethanol dose. The priming injection of ethanol (0.4 g/kg i.g.), which by itself was ineffective to establish a significant CPP before the test, resulted in complete reinstatement of CPP in the group that had acquired a significant place preference reaction after conditioning with 0.8-g/kg ethanol dose. However, in animals primed with tap water (i.g.) there was no increase in time spent in the compartment previously associated with ethanol. Both nociceptin (i.c.v), Ro 64-6198 (0.3 mg/kg i.p.), and naloxone strongly inhibited the “priming” effect of the noncontingent administration of ethanol (p < 0.01 for nociceptin and Ro 64-6198 andp < 0.05 for naloxone, comparison between post-test 1 and post-test 3). In all groups treated with nociceptin and Ro 64-6198, a significant reduction (p < 0.05) of the number of crossings between compartments was observed (compared with the corresponding control groups treated with CSF/vehicle). Treatment with naloxone failed to influence the motor activity of mice.

Effects of Acute and Subchronic Treatment with Ro 64-6198 on Spontaneous Locomotor Activity in Mice

Effects of Acute Treatment with Ro 64-6198 (Fig.7, left column).

In animals treated with vehicle (n = 8), there was a gradual decrease of motor activity over time and an almost complete elimination of rearing, motility, and locomotion after 1 h of habituation to the activity cages. Ro 64-6198 caused a significant influence on rearing -F(3,39) = 3.8, p < 0.05] and motility -F(3,39) = 4.7, p < 0.01]. A significant time effect for rearing, motility, and locomotion [F(5,195) = 61.7, p < 0.0001; 143.5,p < 0.001 and 83.6, p < 0.001, respectively] and a significant dose × time interaction for all parameters [F(15,195) = 1.9, p < 0.05, 1.7, p < 0.05 and 2.1, p = 0.01, respectively] were also found. The post hoc Newman-Keuls test, analyzed for each time point separately, revealed that there was a significant (p < 0.01) lower level of rearing 20 and 30 min after treatment with Ro 64-6198 at the dose of 1 mg/kg (with respect to vehicle-treated group) motility and locomotion 20, 30, and 40 min after treatment with 1 mg/kg Ro 64-6198.

Figure 7
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 7

Effects of acute and subchronic treatment with Ro 64-6198 on spontaneous locomotor activity in mice. Acute administration column: influence of acute administration of Ro 64-6198 on motor activity in nonhabituated mice. The agent was injected i.p. just before placement of the animals in the activity cages. The data represent the cumulative counts for rearing, motility and locomotion in 10-min intervals (mean values and standard errors, n = 8/group). For statistical significance, see Results. Subchronic administration column: drug and experimentally naı̈ve mice were treated twice a day (10:00 AM and 6:00 PM) i.p. for 4 days with either vehicle or Ro 64-6198 (1 mg/kg). On day 5, mice were randomly divided into three treatment groups (n = 8): Subchronic treatment with saline + acute treatment with Ro 64-6198 (Sal-Ro). Subchronic treatment with Ro 64-6198 + acute treatment with Ro 64-6198 (1 mg/kg) (ro-ro). Subchronic treatment with saline + acute treatment with saline (sal-sal). The agent was injected s.c. just before placement of the animals in the activity cages. The data represent the cumulative counts for rearing, motility, and locomotion in 10-min intervals (mean values and standard errors,n = 8/group). For statistical significance, seeResults.

Effects of Subchronic Treatment with Ro 64-6198 on the Locomotor Inhibitory Effect of Ro 64-6198 (Fig. 7, right column).

One-way ANOVA revealed significant group × time interaction for rearing, motility, and locomotion [F(10,105) = 2.4,p < 0.05; 3.7, p < 0.01; and 2.1,p < 0.05, respectively]. Subsequent post hoc analysis revealed that only the group that received subchronic treatment with saline and acute administration of Ro 64-6198 (sal-ro group) displayed significantly lower rearing, motility and locomotion at 10 to 40 min after treatment (p < 0.05, compared with group sal-sal). Subchronic treatment with Ro 64-6198 (1 mg/kg i.p.) did not cause a significant inhibition (compared with the control group sal-sal) of the locomotor activity.

Discussion

The present experiments have demonstrated that 1) intragastric administration of ethanol, paired with a distinct environment, can produce CPP in mice. The CPP can be extinguished by repeated exposure to the place conditioning environment in an ethanol-free state. 2) A priming administration of a low dose of ethanol, which in an alcohol-naive animal failed to induce significant place preference, reinstated the previously acquired and extinguished ethanol-induced CPP. 3) Nociceptin blocked the acquisition of ethanol-induced CPP in line with previous findings, a property shared by the synthetic nociceptin agonist Ro 64-6198. For the first time it was demonstrated that both nociceptin and the synthetic ORL1 agonist could block the expression and ethanol-primed reinstatement of CPP. 4) The nonselective opioid antagonist naloxone failed to block the acquisition of ethanol-induced CPP but inhibited the expression and ethanol-induced priming of the acquired CPP at the dose relevant for opioid receptor blockade.

There is a large body of experimental reports demonstrating reliable ethanol-induced CPP in inbred and outbred mice (Cunningham et al., 1991; Crabbe et al., 1992; Cunningham, 1995; Risinger and Oakes, 1996; Bormann and Cunningham, 1997). In the present study, ethanol at the dose of 0.8 g/kg produced maximal place conditioning effect in outbred NMRI mice. This dose is somewhat lower than those previously reported to induce of CPP in mice. Cunningham et al. (1992) reported a maximal conditioning effect with the doses 3 and 4 g/kg using DBA/2J mice, whereas Risinger et al. (1996) demonstrated significant CPP in Swiss-Webster mice with ethanol doses 1 and 2 g/kg. The higher potency of ethanol in our study compared with the previous reports might be explained by the shorter conditioning trials used in the present study (20 min) compared with 30 and 60 min in the cited study (Risinger and Oakes, 1996).

Comparing pronociceptin gene KO and WT mice we failed to observe significant differences in the acquisition of ethanol-induced CPP. This suggests that during basal conditions nociceptin systems probably play a minor role in modulating the motivational effects of ethanol. However, the increased reactivity of KO mice (as indicated by the increased number of crossings between compartments in the pretest) was noted, which supports previous observations about the higher reactivity to novelty in nociceptin-deficient mice (Reinscheid et al., 1999).

In animals, administration of the originally self-administered drug was shown to be an effective cue for reinstatement of drug-taking behavior (Stewart and Wise, 1992). Here, it was found that repeated daily testing in the absence of unconditioned stimuli lead to a reduction of ethanol-induced CPP, supporting earlier observations obtained in mice with cocaine-induced CPP (Mueller and Stewart, 2000). However, after a priming administration of a low dose of ethanol, the animals chose the environment associated with the ethanol-related stimuli indicative of the reinstatement of the extinguished place preference by the priming dose of ethanol.

Examination of the motivational effects of nociceptin and the synthetic nociceptin receptor agonist Ro 64-6198 revealed that both substances are devoid of either reinforcing or aversive properties in mice, supporting previous observations in rats (Devine et al., 1996a;Ciccocioppo et al., 1999). However, when nociceptin and Ro 64-6198 were tested versus the effect of ethanol in the place-conditioning paradigm, they significantly reduced both the acquisition, expression, and ethanol-induced reinstatement of CPP. Moreover, the combined administration of nociceptin/Ro 64-6198 and ethanol produced a clear-cut place aversion. The fact that nociceptin/Ro 64-6198 can modulate the expression and reinstatement of the conditioned reinforcing effects of ethanol is of special importance. The conditioned positive motivational responses are thought to underlie “craving” in humans, and possibly contribute to relapse to drinking in abstinent alcoholics (Stewart et al., 1984). It could be argued that the effects of nociceptin receptor ligands seem not to be due to unspecific changes in behavior. Acute nociceptin is known to suppress locomotor activity in rats (Devine et al., 1996b) and mice (Reinscheid et al., 1995), although at low doses (lower than 1 nmol/mouse) nociceptin is reported to increase locomotor activity, probably via an “anxiolytic” action (Jenck et al., 1997; Florin et al., 1996). In our experiments, we confirmed that both nociceptin (5 nmol/mouse) and Ro 64-6198 (1 mg/kg) reduced locomotor activity after acute administration. However, tolerance developed very quickly to this effect and already after three injections we failed to observe a significant reduction of locomotor activity as indicated by the number of transitions between the compartments. This observation is in line with the previous data obtained in rats (Devine et al., 1996b;Ciccocioppo et al., 1999).

At present, the neurochemical mechanisms underlying the action of nociceptin and Ro 64-6198 on the rewarding effects of ethanol and reinstatement of ethanol-induced CPP are unknown. Nociceptin has been shown to decrease basal (Murphy and Maidment, 1999) and morphine-stimulated (Di Giannuario and Pieretti, 2000) extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens. This action of nociceptin is probably due to an activation of GABA-ergic interneurons, which exert inhibitory control of mesolimbic dopaminergic neurons. It is also possible that nociceptin can modulate the enkephalinergic fibers known to project to the VTA and synapse with GABA neurons (Sesack and Pickel, 1995). In addition, nociceptin has been shown to decrease glutamate release in cortical slices (Nicol et al., 1996).

Because the endogenous opioids seem to be essential for initiation and maintenance of excessive alcohol consumption it was of interest to include the opioid antagonist naloxone in the present study. Naloxone failed to inhibit acquisition of ethanol-induced CPP at the doses that are relevant to the opioid receptor blockade (0.1 and 1.0 mg/kg), consistent with previously published data (Cunningham et al., 1995). However, at the highest dose tested (10 mg/kg) naloxone completely blocked acquisition of CPP. The disturbances of the mice behavior with the 10-mg/kg dose of naloxone are indicative of unspecific toxic effects of naloxone, probably mediated via nonopioid mechanisms. At doses 1 mg/kg and higher naloxone produced conditioned place aversion, in line with previous observations (Cunningham et al., 1995). In our experiments, we unexpectedly found that naloxone could block the expression of ethanol induced CPP at the doses 1 mg/kg and higher. These doses of naloxone are known to nonselectively block all types of opioid receptors (Pitts et al., 1996) and this probably indicates that multiple opioid systems are involved in the acquisition and expression of ethanol-induced CPP. This observation contrasts with some previous reports (Cunningham et al., 1995, 1998; Bormann and Cunningham, 1997) but is in line with the findings of Middaugh and Bandy (2000). The discrepancies in the results using naloxone might be explained by differences in experimental approaches (biased versus unbiased procedure, different conditional cues, different duration of the conditioning trials as well as different strains of mice used). In our experiments naloxone (1 mg/kg) also strongly inhibited the reinstatement of ethanol-induced CPP primed by the low dose of ethanol (0.4 g/kg). This is, to our knowledge, the first demonstration of the “antirelapse” activity of an opioid antagonist in experiments with mice.

The results of these experiments can also be interpreted in terms of a learning impairment or a retrieval deficit after treatment with nociceptin agonists. In fact, nociceptin has been found to impair hippocampal-dependent spatial learning (Higgins et al., 2002). Thus, it seems possible that nociceptin and its synthetic agonist can block the formation of the learned association between the effects of ethanol and environmental cues. In the reinstatement experiments, nociceptin agonists may prevent memory retrieval mediated by a priming dose of ethanol. Stimulation of nociceptin receptors may prevent the reactivation of memory circuits by subthreshold stimuli (e.g., low doses of ethanol or conditional environmental stimuli). Needless to say, this hypothesis requires experimental confirmation.

In conclusion, the present results support the hypothesis that stimulation of nociceptin (ORL1) receptors reduces the reinforcing and motivational effects of ethanol in mice as well as reinstatement of preference for place induced by ethanol. This suggests that drugs targeted to this receptor may have therapeutic potential in the treatment of alcohol-seeking behavior and relapse.

Acknowledgments

We thank Drs. F. Jenck and J. Wichmann from F. Hoffmann-La Roche for the generous gifts of nociceptin knockout mice and the synthetic nociceptin agonist Ro 64-6198. Portions of these data were presented at the International Narcotic Research Club (INRC) 2001 Congress as a poster report (Helsinki, July 2001).

Footnotes

  • This project received financial support from The Marcus and Amalia Wallenberg Foundation, the Swedish Medical Research Council (project K2001-04X-11588-06A) and Karolinska Institutet.

  • DOI: 10.1124/jpet.102.041350

  • Abbreviations:
    CPP
    conditioned place preference
    ORL1
    opioid receptor-like receptor 1
    Ro 64-6198
    (1S,3aS)-8-(2,3,3a,4,5,6-hexahydro-1H-phenalen-1-yl)-1-phenyl-1,3,8-triaza-spiro[4.5]decan-4-one hydrochloride
    OFQ
    orphanin FQ
    CSF
    cerebrospinal fluid
    i.g.
    intragastric
    ANOVA
    analysis of variance
    KO
    knockout
    WT
    wild-type
    • Received July 9, 2002.
    • Accepted September 23, 2002.
  • The American Society for Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics

References

  1. ↵
    1. Bormann NM,
    2. Cunningham CL
    (1997) The effects of naloxone on expression and acquisition of ethanol place conditioning in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 58:975–982.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Calo G,
    2. Guerrini R,
    3. Rizzi A,
    4. Salvadori S,
    5. Regoli D
    (2000) Pharmacology of nociceptin and its receptor: a novel therapeutic target. Br J Pharmacol 129:1261–1283.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Carroll ME,
    2. Comer SD
    (1996) Animal models of relapse. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 4:11–18.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  4. ↵
    1. Ciccocioppo R,
    2. Angeletti S,
    3. Panocka I,
    4. Massi M
    (2000) Nociceptin/orphanin FQ and drugs of abuse. Peptides 21:1071–1080.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Ciccocioppo R,
    2. Panocka I,
    3. Polidori C,
    4. Regoli D,
    5. Massi M
    (1999) Effect of nociceptin on alcohol intake in alcohol-preferring rats. Psychopharmacology 141:220–224.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Crabbe JC,
    2. Phillips TJ,
    3. Cunningham CL,
    4. Belknap JK
    (1992) Genetic determinants of ethanol reinforcement. Ann NY Acad Sci 654:302–310.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Cunningham CL
    (1995) Localization of genes influencing ethanol-induced conditioned place preference and locomotor activity in BXD recombinant inbred mice. Psychopharmacology 120:28–41.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Cunningham CL,
    2. Dickinson SD,
    3. Okorn DM
    (1995) Naloxone facilitates extinction but does not affect acquisition of ethanol-induced conditioned place preference. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 3:330–343.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  9. ↵
    1. Cunningham CL,
    2. Hallett CL,
    3. Niehus DR,
    4. Hunter JS,
    5. Nouth L,
    6. Risinger FO
    (1991) Assessment of ethanol's hedonic effects in mice selectively bred for sensitivity to ethanol-induced hypothermia. Psychopharmacology 105:84–92.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Cunningham CL,
    2. Henderson CM,
    3. Bormann NM
    (1998) Extinction of ethanol-induced conditioned place preference and conditioned place aversion: effects of naloxone. Psychopharmacology 139:62–70.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Cunningham CL,
    2. Niehus DR,
    3. Malott DH,
    4. Prather LK
    (1992) Genetic differences in the rewarding and activating effects of morphine and ethanol. Psychopharmacology 107:385–393.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Devine DP,
    2. Reinscheid RK,
    3. Monsma FJ, Jr,
    4. Civelli O,
    5. Akil H
    (1996a) The novel neuropeptide orphanin FQ fails to produce conditioned place preference or aversion. Brain Res 727:225–229.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Devine DP,
    2. Taylor L,
    3. Reinscheid RK,
    4. Monsma FJ, Jr,
    5. Civelli O,
    6. Akil H
    (1996b) Rats rapidly develop tolerance to the locomotor-inhibiting effects of the novel neuropeptide orphanin FQ. Neurochem Res 21:1387–1396.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Di Giannuario A,
    2. Pieretti S
    (2000) Nociceptin differentially affects morphine-induced dopamine release from the nucleus accumbens and nucleus caudate in rats. Peptides 27:1125–1130.
  15. ↵
    1. Florin S,
    2. Suaudeau C,
    3. Meunier JC,
    4. Costentin J
    (1996) Nociceptin stimulates locomotion and exploratory behavior in mice. Eur J Pharm 317:9–13.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Franklin KBJ,
    2. Paxinos GT
    (1997) The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. Academic Press, San Diego.
  17. ↵
    1. Froehlich JC,
    2. Li TK
    (1994) Opioid involvement in alcohol drinking. Ann NY Acad Sci 739:156–167.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Higgins GA,
    2. Kew JN,
    3. Richards JG,
    4. Takeshima H,
    5. Jenck F,
    6. Adam G,
    7. Wichmann J,
    8. Kemp JA,
    9. Grottick AJ
    (2002) A combined pharmacological and genetic approach to investigate the role of orphanin FQ in learning and memory. Eur J Neurosci 15:911–922.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Jenck F,
    2. Moreau JL,
    3. Martin JR,
    4. Kilpatrick GJ,
    5. Reinscheid RK,
    6. Monsma FJ,
    7. Nothacker HP,
    8. Civelli O
    (1997) Orphanin FQ acts as an anxiolitic to attenuate behavioral responses to stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94:14854–14858.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  20. ↵
    1. Jenck F,
    2. Wichmann J,
    3. Dautzenberg FM,
    4. Moreau JL,
    5. Ouagazzal AM,
    6. Martin JR,
    7. Lundstrom K,
    8. Cesura AM,
    9. Poli SM,
    10. Roever S,
    11. et al.
    (2000) A synthetic agonist at the orphanin FQ/nociceptin receptor ORL1: anxiolytic profile in the rat. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97:4938–4943.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  21. ↵
    1. Koster A,
    2. Montkowski A,
    3. Schulz S,
    4. Stube EM,
    5. Knaudt K,
    6. Jenck F,
    7. Moreau JL,
    8. Nothacker HP,
    9. Civelli O,
    10. Reinscheid RK
    (1999) Targeted disruption of the orphanin FQ/nociceptin gene increases stress susceptibility and impairs stress adaptation in mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96:10444–10449.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    1. Martin-Fardon R,
    2. Ciccocioppo R,
    3. Massi M,
    4. Weiss F
    (2000) Nociceptin prevents stress-induced ethanol but not cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Neuroreport 11:1939–1943.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Meunier JC,
    2. Mollereau C,
    3. Toll L,
    4. Suaudeau C,
    5. Moisand C,
    6. Alvinerie P,
    7. Butour JL,
    8. Guillemot JC,
    9. Ferrara P,
    10. Monserrat B,
    11. et al.
    (1995) Isolation and structure of the endogenous agonist of opioid-receptor-like ORL1 receptor. Nature (Lond) 377:532–535.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Middaugh LD,
    2. Bandy AL
    (2000) Naltrexone effects on ethanol consumption and response to ethanol conditioned cues in C57BL/6 mice. Psychopharmacology 151:321–327.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Mueller D,
    2. Stewart J
    (2000) Cocaine-induced conditioned place preference: reinstatement by priming injections of cocaine after extinction. Behav Brain Res 115:39–47.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Murphy NP,
    2. Maidment NT
    (1999) Orphanin FQ/nociceptin modulation of mesolimbic dopamine transmission determined by microdialysis. J Neurochem 73:179–186.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Nicol B,
    2. Lambert DG,
    3. Rowbotham DJ,
    4. Smart D,
    5. McKnight AT
    (1996) Nociceptin induced inhibition of K+-evoked glutamate release from rat cerebrocortical slices. Br J Pharmacol 119:1081–1083.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. O'Brien CP
    (1975) Experimental analysis of conditioning factors in human narcotic addiction. Pharmacol Rev 27:533–543.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Ögren SO,
    2. Hall H,
    3. Köhler C,
    4. Magnusson O,
    5. Sjostrand SE
    (1986) The selective dopamine D2 receptor antagonist raclopride discriminates between dopamine-mediated motor functions. Psychopharmacology 90:287–294.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Pitts RC,
    2. West JP,
    3. Morgan D,
    4. Dykstra LA,
    5. Picker MJ
    (1996) Opioids and rate of positively reinforced behavior: differential antagonism by naltrexone. Behav Pharmacol 7:205–215.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Reinscheid RK,
    2. Nothacker HP,
    3. Bourson A,
    4. Ardati A,
    5. Henningsen RA,
    6. Bunzow JR,
    7. Grandy DK,
    8. Langen H,
    9. Monsma FJ,
    10. Civelli O
    (1995) Orphanin FQ: a neuropeptide that activates an opioid-like G-protein-coupled receptor. Science (Wash DC) 270:792–794.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    1. Reinscheid RK,
    2. Koster A,
    3. Knaudt K,
    4. Montkowski A,
    5. Civelli O
    (1999) Increased stress susceptibility and fear in orphanin FQ/nociceptin knockout mice is dependent on chronic social stress. Soc Neurosci Abstr 25:1473.
    OpenUrl
  33. ↵
    1. Risinger FO,
    2. Bormann NM,
    3. Oakes RA
    (1996) Reduced sensitivity to ethanol reward, but not ethanol aversion, in mice lacking 5-HT1B receptors. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 20:1401–1405.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Risinger FO,
    2. Oakes RA
    (1996) Dose- and conditioning-trial dependent ethanol-induced conditioned place preference in Swiss-Webster mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 55:117–123.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Sesack SR,
    2. Pickel VM
    (1995) Ultrastructural relationships between terminals immunoreactive for enkephalin, GABA, or both transmitters in the rat ventral tegmental area. Brain Res 672:261–275.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Stewart J,
    2. Wise RA
    (1992) Reinstatement of heroin self-administration habits. Morphine prompts and naltrexone discourages renewed responding after extinction. Psychopharmacology 108:79–84.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Stewart J,
    2. de Wit H,
    3. Eikelboom R
    (1984) Role of unconditioned and conditioned drug effects in the self-administration of opiates and stimulants. Psychol Rev 91:251–268.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Tzschentke TM
    (1998) Measuring reward with the conditioned place preference paradigm: a comprehensive review of drug effects, recent progress and new issues. Prog Neurobiol 56:613–672.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Wagner EJ,
    2. Ronnekleiv OK,
    3. Grandy DK,
    4. Kelly MJ
    (1998) The peptide orphanin FQ inhibits beta-endorphin neurons and neurosecretory cells in the hypothalamic arcuate nucleus by activating an inwardly rectifying K+ conductance. Neuroendocrinology 67:73–82.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Wichmann J,
    2. Adam G,
    3. Rover S,
    4. Hennig M,
    5. Scalone M,
    6. Cesura AM,
    7. Dautzenberg FM,
    8. Jenck F
    (2000) Synthesis of (1S,3aS)-8-(2,3,3a,4,5,6-hexahydro-1H-phenalen-1-yl)-1-phenyl-1,3,8-triaza-spiro[4.5]decan-4-one, a potent and selective orphanin FQ (OFQ) receptor agonist with anxiolytic-like properties. Eur J Med Chem 35:839–851.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
View Abstract
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics: 304 (1)
Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
Vol. 304, Issue 1
1 Jan 2003
  • Table of Contents
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
Download PDF
Article Alerts
Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Email Article

Thank you for sharing this Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Acquisition, Expression, and Reinstatement of Ethanol-Induced Conditioned Place Preference in Mice: Effects of Opioid Receptor-Like 1 Receptor Agonists and Naloxone
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics.
Citation Tools
Research ArticleBEHAVIORAL PHARMACOLOGY

Acquisition, Expression, and Reinstatement of Ethanol-Induced Conditioned Place Preference in Mice: Effects of Opioid Receptor-Like 1 Receptor Agonists and Naloxone

A. Kuzmin, J. Sandin, L. Terenius and S. O. Ögren
Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics January 1, 2003, 304 (1) 310-318; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1124/jpet.102.041350

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero

Share
Research ArticleBEHAVIORAL PHARMACOLOGY

Acquisition, Expression, and Reinstatement of Ethanol-Induced Conditioned Place Preference in Mice: Effects of Opioid Receptor-Like 1 Receptor Agonists and Naloxone

A. Kuzmin, J. Sandin, L. Terenius and S. O. Ögren
Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics January 1, 2003, 304 (1) 310-318; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1124/jpet.102.041350
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • eLetters
  • PDF

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Targeting dopamine D2, adenosine A2A, and glutamate mGlu5 receptors to reduce repetitive behaviors in deer mice
  • Methocinnamox and Heroin Respiratory Depression
  • N-Palmitoylethanolamide exerts antidepressant-like effects in rats: involvement of PPAR-alpha pathway in the hippocampus
Show more Behavioral Pharmacology

Similar Articles

  • Home
  • Alerts
Facebook   Twitter   LinkedIn   RSS

Navigate

  • Current Issue
  • Fast Forward by date
  • Fast Forward by section
  • Latest Articles
  • Archive
  • Search for Articles
  • Feedback
  • ASPET

More Information

  • About JPET
  • Editorial Board
  • Instructions to Authors
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Customized Alerts
  • RSS Feeds
  • Subscriptions
  • Permissions
  • Terms & Conditions of Use

ASPET's Other Journals

  • Drug Metabolism and Disposition
  • Molecular Pharmacology
  • Pharmacological Reviews
  • Pharmacology Research & Perspectives
ISSN 1521-0103 (Online)

Copyright © 2019 by the American Society for Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics